Cerebral
small vessel disease (SVD) is a group of clinical, neuroimaging, and
pathological processes with various etiologies that affect the small arteries
and capillaries of the brain.1 Hypertension-related cerebral small
vessel diseases and cerebral amyloid angiopathy are the most common forms. It
has been postulated that cerebral microvascular disease contributes to vascular
dementia and vascular cognitive impairment. 1 Most of the published
studies assessed cerebral small vessel
disease associated with vascular dementia, ,on the contrary, few studies
assessed the cerebral small vessel disease associated with vascular cognitive
impairment.3 It is estimated
that older adults with small vessel disease develop vascular dementia between 5
and 25 times more than age-matched individuals.2 The pattern of cognitive
impairment found in cerebral SVD differs from that found in Alzheimer’s disease
where deficits of episodic memory, which is the hallmark of Alzheimer’s
disease, are often mild or even absent
in SVD associated cognitive impairment.4 Cognitive changes from
cerebral small vessel disease (SVD)
generally
include impairment of attention and working memory, executive functions,
processing speed, and memory performance i.e., primarily encoding and retrieval
rather than retention. 5,6,7,8,9, Most established brief cognitive
assessment tools, such as the mini-mental state examination (MMSE), 10
have been developed in the context of Alzheimer’s disease. These tools are insensitive
even to the gross impairment of the executive functions.11 The
aim of this work is to evaluate the cognitive functions in patients with SVD and its correlation with
the findings of the brain imaging.
SUBJECTS
AND METHODS
Subjects
This case-control study
was conducted on forty Egyptian patients (Group I) whose age ranged between
50-70 years (mean ± SD = 61.43±6.25). They were diagnosed to have small vessel
disease clinically and by using brain magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) criteria
for lacunar syndromes and diffuse white matter abnormality (Fazekas’ scale)12.
Patients were enrolled from Neurology clinic and Neurology department, Kasr El-Aini
Hospital, Cairo University,
during the period between January 2013 to March 2014. Ten healthy volunteers
matching the patient group in age, sex and educational level were also enrolled
as a control group (Group II). We had
included patients with neurological symptoms and signs of lacunar infarction
clinical syndromes who were able to read and write, and do simple calculations.
We had excluded patients whose age were below 50 years, patients with large
vessel disease either by neurological signs or MRI evidence of cortical
infarction, patients with an evidence of cardioembolic source, patients
presenting with dementia or MMSE scores below 24, patients with an evidence of
medical systemic disease that can affect the cognitive function (e.g. hepatic
encephalopathy, uremic encephalopathy, septicemia, diabetic coma or
hypothyroidism), those with history of chronic drug intake or alcohol
consumption that may affect the cognitive function, patients with severe
dysphasia, severe hearing or visual impairment affecting ability to complete
testing, patients presenting with other causes of small vessel disease like
CADASIL, vasculitis,...etc., and those with any medical condition that
interfere with MRI.
METHODS
All patients in
group (I) and healthy volunteers in control group (group II) were subjected to
thorough neurological assessment and neuropsychological tests including global
cognitive functioning and specific cognitive functions tests.
Neuropsychometric Assessment
A. Assessment
of global cognitive functioning included:
Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) 10
with total score 30 (patients with score below 24 were excluded from the study)
to exclude dementia patients. We also used Addenbrooke's cognitive examination
(ACE-R) Final Revised Version A (2005) which includes the MMSE in addition to
assessment of the executive function and visuospatial skills with a total score
100, scores above 82 exclude dementia. 12 We used the translated
Arabic version, which is valid for assessment of dementia in Arabic speaking
people. 13
B. Assessment
of specific cognitive functions included:
a. Assessment
of verbal memory: using Paired Associate Learning Test (PALT) which uses the
concept of semantic cueing for assessment of verbal memory14
b. Assessment of language skills:
i. Verbal
Fluency (animals) Test (VFT): The semantic subtest has also been shown to be
quite effective in measuring executive functioning and language ability. This
may be because the subtest seems to require a higher level of thought processes
since people have to think of meaning rather than just beginning sounds of
words.15 Scores below 17 indicates concern.
ii. Token
test: The short version used in this study contains 36 commands (scored one
if correct and 0 if incorrect). The test is divided into six parts. The score
is calculated by assigning 1 point for each item answered completely correct,
ranging from zero to 36 points. The cut-off score was 29.
The pieces are arranged in a specific order and the subject must answer exactly
as the item requests.16
c. Assessment
of praxis: We used block design subtest, which is one of the performance
Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scales (WAIS) for assessment of visuospatial abilities. The cut off score
is 21.17
d. Assessment
of executive functions:
i. Trail
making: to test for visual attention and task switching. The test consists of
two parts : part A to make a trail between 25 circles numbered from 1 to 25 as
fast as possible, part B to make a trail between 25 circles numbered from 1 to
13 and alphabetic from (أ ) to
( س ). Cut off score more than five minutes.
ii. Digit
symbol tests (DST): The subject is showed numbers from 1-9 and different
symbols attached to each number. The subject try to remember them and then to
recall in the test. the score scaled on Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scales.17
All neuropsychological tests were
performed in a face -to-face interview in a quite examination room with a break
for about ten minutes after Addenbrooke's test and five minutes- break after
each test.
C. Beck
Inventory for measuring depression:
It includes screening questions for
depression by self-report statements. It is especially useful in assessing
depression in patients with neurological disease. 18
MRI Brain:
Axial T1-, T2-, and
proton density-weighted cerebral MR scans on a 1.5-Tesla SIEMENS scanner were
used to assess patients and control. The Fazeka's scale was used which rates
periventricular hyperintensities (PVHIs) and diffuse white matter hyperintensities
(DWMHIs) on T2/PD-weighted images on a 4-grade scale; Fazekas 0: None or a
single punctuate WMH lesion, Fazekas 1: Multiple punctuate lesions, Fazekas 2:
Beginning confluency of lesions (bridging), Fazekas 3: Large confluent lesions.
19
Routine Laboratory Tests
Both groups were tested
for fasting blood sugar, 2 hours postprandial sugar, complete blood count,
estimated sedimentation rate, kidney function test urea, creatinine and uric
acid, liver function tests (AST and ALT), serum electrolytes, lipid profile
(Cholesterol ,Triglycerides, HDL and LDL).
ECG and Trans-thoracic Echocardiography
To exclude arrhythmias.
Statistical Methods
Analysis of data was
done using SPSS (statistical program for social science version 12) as follows:
Description of quantitative variables as mean, S.D. and range, description of
qualitative variables as numbers and percentage, Chi-square test was used to
compare qualitative variables between groups. Fisher exact test was used
instead of Chi-square test when one or more expected cells<5. Spearman
correlation coefficient test was used to rank different variables versus each
other positively or inversely. Unpaired t-test was used for comparison of
quantitative variables between two independent groups in parametric data
(S.D.< 50%) mean. Mann Whitney Wilcoxon U test was used instead of unpaired
t-test in parametric data. P-value: >0.05 insignificant, < 0.05
significant and < 0.01 highly significant.
RESULTS
1.
Sample characteristics:
Demographics and vascular risk factors
for both groups are shown in Table 1, As
indicated in the Table, all of the participants in group I were diabetic and
hypertensive, 28% of them had ECG changes suggestive of IHD while 30% were
smokers.
2.
Comparisons of the cognitive functions
tests:
Table (2) presents the mean scores (±SD)
for the cognitive functions tests for the two groups. As can be seen in Table
2, the performance of the patient group (group I) was lower than group II
in Addenbrooke test, verbal fluency
test, trail making B test, DST, PALT and block design test and these statistical differences were highly
significant (P value <0.01), a statistically significant difference between the 2 groups was also found in trail
making A test (P value <0.05), however, no statistical significant
difference was found between the performance of the two groups as regard the token test (P value
> 0.05).
3.
Correlation between age and cognitive
functions tests:
Table (3) presents correlation between age and
cognitive functions tests for the two groups. There were a highly statistical
significant negative correlation between age and the performance of the
patients and control groups in Addenbrooke test, trail making test, DST, PALT
and block design test (P value <0.01), and statistical significant negative
correlation between age and the performance of the patients and control groups
in verbal fluency test and block design test (P value <0.05). A statistical
significant negative correlation was found
between age and the performance of the patients in token test (P value
<0.05).
4.
Correlation between duration of Fazekas’
scale, duration of diabetes mellitus (DM), duration of hypertension, and
cognitive functions tests:
Table (4) presents correlation between Fazekas
scale, duration of diabetes mellitus (DM), duration of hypertension, and
cognitive functions tests for the patient group. As Table (4) indicates.
There were a highly statistical significant negative
correlation between Fazekas scale and performance of patients at Addenbrooke
test, trail making B test, DST and block design test (P value <0.01), a
statistically significant negative correlation was also found in verbal fluency
test, trail making A test, and PALT (P value <0.05). Such statistically
significant negative correlation was not found in token test (P value˃0.05).
There were a highly statistical significant negative correlation between
duration of DM and performance of patients at trail making B test, DST and
block design test (P value <0.01), moreover, a statistically significant negative
correlation between duration of DM and performance of patients was found in
Addenbrooke test, verbal fluency test, trail making A test, PALT and token test
(P value <0.05). There were a highly statistical significant negative
correlation between duration of hypertension and performance of patients at
block design test, trail making A test, trail making B test and DST (P value
<0.01), a statistically significant negative correlation between duration of
hypertension and performance of patients was also found in Addenbrooke test,
verbal fluency test, PALT and token test (P value <0.05).
Table 1.
Demographics and vascular risk factors for patients and control groups.
|
Patients
group
(Group
I)
|
Control
group
(Group
II)
|
P-value
|
Demographic
data:
Age (Mean±SD)
|
61.43±6.25
|
59.55±5.25
|
0.12
|
Sex: Male
|
23(57.5%)
|
6(60%)
|
-
|
Female
|
17(42.5%)
|
4(40%)
|
-
|
Vascular
risk factors:
|
Patients
with HTN (n)
|
40
|
0
|
-
|
Duration
of HTN (years)
|
9.08±4.79
|
0
|
-
|
Patients
with DM (n)
|
40
|
0
|
-
|
Duration
of DM (years) (Mean±SD)
|
7.54±4.43
|
0
|
|
FBS (
mg/dl ) (Mean±SD)
|
127.95 ±51.56
|
86.10±6.12
|
.014
|
Patients
with ECG changes
|
14 (28%)
|
0
|
-
|
Uric
acid (mg/dl) (Mean±SD)
|
5.68 ±0.66
|
5.45±0.58
|
0.312
|
Cholesterol (mg/dl ) (Mean±SD)
|
185.10 ±12.16
|
168.35±21.85
|
0.024*
|
Triglycerides
( mg/dl ) (Mean±SD)
|
99.7±54.4
|
127.8±47.4
|
0.034*
|
LDL(
mg/dl) (Mean±SD)
|
140±15
|
117±29
|
0.044
|
HDL(
mg/dl) (Mean±SD)
|
55±10.3
|
45±10.3
|
0.023*
|
Smokers
(n(%))
|
12(30%)
|
4(40%)
|
-
|
ECG
Electrocardiogram, DM Diabetes mellitus, FBS
Fasting blood sugar, HDL High density lipoprotein, HTN
hypertension, LDL Low
density lipoprotein
*Significant
at P<0.05
Table 2. Comparison of the
scores of the cognitive functions tests
between the patients and control groups.
|
Patients
group
(Group
I)
Mean
± SD
|
Control group
(Group
II)
Mean
± SD
|
t.
test
|
P.
value
|
Addenbrooke
test (ACE-R)
|
92.23±3.60
|
98.80±0.92
|
-5.691
|
<0.01**
|
Verbal
fluency test (0-14)
|
19.35±3.78
|
33.00±2.83
|
-10.664
|
<0.01**
|
Block
design test (0-20)
|
10.43±1.32
|
15.90±1.20
|
-11.945
|
<0.01**
|
Trail
making test part A in seconds
|
12.53±4.32
|
10.30±2.00
|
16.465
|
<0.05*
|
Trail
making test part B in seconds
|
63.88±7.46
|
18.50±3.31
|
18.675
|
<0.01**
|
DST
(0-20)
|
6.75±1.28
|
8.5±0.7
|
-4.16
|
<0.01**
|
PALT
(0-36)
|
12.13±3.00
|
20.40±0.70
|
-8.597
|
<0.01**
|
Token test (0-36)
|
35.95±0.32
|
36±0
|
-0.5
|
> 0.05
|
DST Digit
symbol tests, PALT Paired association learning test.
*Significant
at P<0.05 **Significant at P<0.01
Table 3.
Correlation between age and cognitive functions tests.
|
Age
|
Patients group (Group I)
|
Control group (Group II)
|
Addenbrooke's test
|
r
|
-.094
|
0.423
|
P. value
|
<
0.01**
|
<
0.01**
|
Verbal fluency test
|
r
|
-.269
|
-0.187
|
P. value
|
<
0.05*
|
<
0.05*
|
Block design
|
r
|
-.237
|
-0.182
|
P. value
|
<
0.05*
|
<
0.05*
|
Trail making test part A
|
r
|
0.499
|
-.144
|
P. value
|
<
0.01**
|
<
0.05*
|
Trail making test part B
|
r
|
0.463
|
0.116
|
P. value
|
<
0.01**
|
<
0.05*
|
PALT
|
r
|
-0.406
|
0.236
|
P. value
|
<
0.01**
|
<
0.01**
|
DST
|
r
|
-0.256
|
0.291
|
P. value
|
<
0.01**
|
<
0.01**
|
Token test
|
r
|
-0.222
|
0.244
|
P. value
|
<
0.05*
|
>
0.05
|
DST Digit
symbol tests, PALT Paired association learning test.
*Significant
at P<0.05 **Significant at P<0.01
Table
4. Correlation between MRI Fazekas scale, duration of
DM, duration of hypertension, and cognitive functions tests.
|
MRI
Fazekas scale
|
DM
(Years)
|
HTN
(Years)
|
Addenbrooke's
test
|
r
|
-.885
|
-0.057
|
-0.188
|
P.
value
|
< 0.01**
|
<0.05*
|
< 0.05*
|
Verbal
fluency test
|
r
|
-.665
|
0.129
|
-0.231
|
P.
value
|
< 0.05*
|
< 0.05*
|
< 0.05*
|
Block design test
|
r
|
-.659
|
0.129
|
-0.224
|
P.
value
|
< 0.01**
|
< 0.01**
|
< 0.01**
|
Trail
making test part A
|
r
|
.725
|
0.114
|
0.509
|
P.
value
|
< 0.05*
|
< 0.05*
|
< 0.01**
|
Trail
making test part B
|
r
|
.818
|
-0.009
|
0.457
|
P.
value
|
< 0.01**
|
< 0.01**
|
< 0.01**
|
PALT
|
r
|
-.465
|
-0.005
|
-0.293
|
P.
value
|
< 0.05*
|
< 0.05*
|
< 0.05*
|
DST
|
r
|
-.527
|
-0.361
|
-0.270
|
P.
value
|
< 0.01**
|
< 0.01**
|
< 0.01**
|
Token
test
|
r
|
-.196
|
0.114
|
-0.201
|
DST Digit
symbol tests, HTN hypertension, PALT Paired
association learning test.
*Significant
at P<0.05 **Significant at P<0.01
DISCUSSION
Cerebral small vessel
disease (SVD) is a major cause of stroke, age-related cognitive decline, and
vascular dementia.20 Most of the published studies focused on
vascular dementia (Vad) rather than mild cognitive impairment (MCI) associated
with cerebral small vessel disease. The cognitive impairment in SVD associated
with lacunes may well be due to damage to cortical–subcortical pathways,
disrupting the complex and distributed networks that underpin processes such as
executive function and information processing.21 On cerebral
magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), white matter hyperintensities (WMH) and
lacunes, both of which are frequently observed in the elderly, are generally
viewed as evidence of small vessel disease. 22
Our study was aiming at
detection of the subtle cognitive impairment associated with SVD in non-demented
subjects. we assessed multiple domains of cognition: verbal memory, language
skills, praxis and executive function.
We
faced some obstacles in using Addenbrooke's Cognitive Examination-Revised 2005
(the English version) because some questions, even with highly educated
subjects, were not familiar and inappropriate for Arabic cultures (like the
name of the USA president who was assassinated in the 1960's), also in language
repetition, naming and reading. So we used the version which was translated to
Arabic language in King Khaled University Hospital Neurology section in Riyadh
KSA which is valid for assessment of dementia in Arabic speaking people.13
The total score of Addenbrooke's Cognitive Examination-Revised in patients
group was 92.23±3.6 (mean±SD) while control group was 98.80± 0.92 (mean ± SD)
with p-value (0.001). The Arabic version of ACE-R appears to be a reliable tool
for the assessment of the cognitive impairment associated with small vessel
disease.
Results
of our study revealed a statistically significant difference between the
patients and control groups in the performance of most of the cognitive
function tests. The current findings are going with previous studies that have
shown that cognitive impairment is common among SVD patients.23.24 A
highly statistical significant difference was found between the scores of the
patients and control subjects in trail making test specially part-B, digit
symbol test, and block design test. A statistical significant difference
was found between the scores of the patients and control subjects in Trail
making test part A (P-value < 0.05*). These results signify that executive
functions can be affected in non-demented individuals with SVD. The current
findings are in concordance with previous studies that have shown that cognitive
impairment is common in the SVD patients.23.24, 25
A highly significant statistical
difference was found between the scores of the patients and control subjects in
verbal fluency test, paired associate learning test (PALT) denoting that verbal
memory and language skills are affected as well. This can add to the pattern of
cognitive impairment associated with SVD across various studies that found
mainly impairment of executive function and information processing speed. 23.24,25
We also found that the
duration of the vascular risk factors of the SVD (hypertension and diabetes
mellitus) were negatively correlated with the performance of the patient group
in all of the cognitive tests i.e. the longer the duration of hypertension and
diabetes mellitus the poorer the cognitive performance. These findings have
been supported by other studies that have been designed to assess the magnitude
of cognitive dysfunction in diabetes mellitus. 26,27,28
A similar significant
correlation was found between the grade of Fazekas scale, which were negatively
correlated, with the performance of the patient group in all of the cognitive
testing apart from the token test i.e. patients with higher Fazekas grade had
poorer cognitive performance. These results are consistent with the findings of
many other studies that assessed the correlation between leukoaraiosis and
cognitive decline. 29,30,31
In conclusion, we
found that SVD was associated with poor performance in verbal fluency, language
skills and executive functions. However, since this was a cross-sectional
study, further prospective studies will be needed to elucidate the association
between MRI parameters and vascular cognitive impairment.
[Disclosure: Authors report no conflict
of interest]
REFERENCES
1.
Sierra C.
Cerebral small vessel disease, cognitive impairment and vascular dementia.
Panminerva Med. 2012; 54(3): 179-88.
2.
Mosley TH Jr,
Knopman DS,
Catellier DJ,
Bryan N, Hutchinson RG,
Grothues CA,
et.al. Cerebral MRI findings and cognitive functioning: the Atherosclerosis
Risk in Communities study. 2005; Neurology. 64: 2056–62.
3.
Loeb C, Meyer JS. Vascular dementia: still a debatable
entity? J Neurol Sci. 1996;143:31–40.
4.
Lafosse JM, Reed BR, Mungas D, Sterling SB, Wahbeh H,
Jagust WJ. Fluency and memory differences between ischemic vascular dementia
and Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychol. 1997;11:514–22.
5.
Grau-Olivares M, Arboix A. Mild cognitive impairment
in stroke patients with ischemic cerebral small-vessel disease: a forerunner of
vascular dementia? Expert Rev Neurotherap. 2009; 9:1201–17.
6.
Jokinen H, Kalska H, Ylikoski R, Madureira S, Verdelho
A, van der Flier WM, et al. Longitudinal cognitive decline in subcortical
ischemic vascular disease–the LADIS Study. Cerebrovasc Dis. 2009; 27: 384–91.
7.
Kramer JH, Reed BR, Mungas D, Weiner MW, Chui HC.
Executive dysfunction in subcortical ischaemic vascular disease. J Neurol
Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2002;72:217–20.
8.
Lesage SR, Mosley TH, Wong TY, Szklo M, Knopman D,
Catellier DJ, et al. Retinal
microvascular abnormalities and cognitive decline: the ARIC 14-year follow-up
study. Neurology. 2009;73:862–8.
9.
Prins ND, van Dijk EJ, den Heijer T, Vermeer SE, Jolles
J, Koudstaal PJ, Hofman A, et al.
Cerebral small-vessel disease and decline in information processing
speed, executive function and memory. Brain. 2005;128:2034–41.
10. Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PH.
Mini-mental state: a practical method for grading the cognitive state of
patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res.1975;12:189–98.
11. Lezak DM. The problem of assessing
executive functions. Int J Psychiatry.1982;17:281–97.
12.
Mioshi E, Dawson K, Mitchell J, Arnold R, Hodges JR.
The Addenbrooke’s Cognitive Examination Revised (ACE-R): a brief cognitive test
battery for dementia screening. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry.2006;21:1078–85
13. Al Salman AS. The Saudi Arabian Adaptation
of Addenbrooke's cognitive examination –Revised (Arabic ACE-R) 2013; 3:57-80. [Cited 2014 July 13]. Available
from: http://theses.gla.ac.uk/4706.
14. Wechsler D, Stone C. Wechsler Memory
Scale. New York:
Psychological Corporation; 1973.
15. Poeck K1, Huber W, Willmes K. Outcome of
intensive language treatment in aphasia. J Speech Hear Disord. 1989
Aug;54(3):471-9.
16. de Paula JJ, Schlottfeldt CG, Moreira L,
Cotta M, Bicalho MA, Romano-Silva MA, et al. Psychometric properties of a brief
neuropsychological protocol for use in geriatric populations. Rev Psiq Clín.
2010; 37(6):246-50.
17. Wechsler, D. Manual for the Wechsler Adult
Intelligence Scale- Revised. New York: Psychological Corporation; 1981.
18. Beck AT, Ward CH, Mendelson M, Mock J,
Erbaugh J. An inventory for measuring depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1961; 4:
561-71.
19.
Fazekas F, Chawluk JB, Alavi A, Hurtig HI, Zimmerman
RA. MR signal abnormalities at 1.5 T in Alzheimer's dementia and normal aging.
AJR Am J Roentgenol. 1987;149 (2): 351-6.
20. Roman GC, Erkinjuntti T, Wallin A, Pantoni
L, Chui HC. Subcortical ischaemic
vascular dementia. Lancet Neurol. 2002;1:426–436.
21. Bressler SL, Menon V. Large-scale brain
networks in cognition: emerging methods and principles. Trends Cogn Sci.
2010;14:277–90.
22. van Swieten JC, van den Hout JH, van Ketel
BA, Hijdra A, Wokke JH, van Gijn J. Periventricular lesions in the white matter
on magnetic resonance imaging in the elderly. A morphometric correlation with
arteriolosclerosis and dilated perivascular spaces. Brain. 1991; 114: 761–74.
23. O’Sullivan M, Morris RG, Markus HS. Brief
cognitive assessment for patients with cerebral small vessel disease. J Neurol Neurosurg
Psychiatry. 2005; 76: 1140.
24. O'Brien JT, Erkinjuntti T, Reisberg B,Roman G,,Sawada T, Pantoni L, et.al. Vascular cognitive impairment.
Lancet Neurol. 2003; 2: 89–98.
25. Charlton RA, Morris RG, Nitkunan A, Markus
HS. The cognitive profiles of CADASIL and sporadic small vessel disease.
Neurology 2006; 66: 1523.
26. Ryan CM, Geckle MO, Orchard TJ. Cognitive efficiency
declines over time in adults with type 1 diabetes: effects of micro- and
macrovascular complications. Diabetologia. 2003; 46:940–8.
27. Messier C. Impact of impaired glucose tolerance and type 2
diabetes on cognitive aging. Neurobiol Aging. 2005;26 (1):26–30.
28. Brands AM, Kessels RP, Hoogma RP, Henselmans JM, van der
Beek Boter JW, et al. Cognitive performance, psychological well-being, and
brain magnetic resonance imaging in older patients with type 1 diabetes.
Diabetes. 2006; 55:1800–6.
29. Schmidt R, Petrovic K,
Ropele S, Enzinger C,
Fazekas F.
Progression of leukoaraiosis and cognition. Stroke. 2007;38(9):2619-25.
30. Schmidt R, Scheltens P,
Erkinjuntti T,
Pantoni L, Markus HS, Wallin A, et al. White matter lesion progression: a surrogate
endpoint for trials in cerebral small-vessel disease. Neurology. 2004 13;63(1):139-44.
31. Pavlovic AM,
Pekmezovic T,
Tomic G, Trajkovic JZ,
Sternic N.
Baseline predictors of cognitive decline in patients with cerebral small vessel
disease. J Alzheimers
Dis. 2014 1;42(0):S37-43.
الملخص العربي
اعتلال الأوعية الدماغية الصغري والمعرفية
يعتبر مرض اعتلال الأوعية
الدموية الدماغية الصغري مجموعة من العمليات المرضية التي تؤثر على الشرايين
الصغيرة والشعيرات الدموية الدقيقة في المخ وقد تؤدي إلى اختلال الوظائف الذهنية والمعرفية
وقد تفضي إلى الخرف الوعائي. وقد أجري هذا البحث لتقييم الاضطرابات في الوظائف
المعرفية في مرضي الأوعية الدماغية الصغيرة وربطها بنتائج أشعة الرنين المغناطيسي على
المخ.
وقد اشتملت الدراسة على خمسون شخصاً وقد تم تقسيم هؤلاء الأشخاص
إلى مجموعتين:
المجموعة الأولي
(مجموعه الدراسة): اشتملت على أربعين شخصاً مصابين بمرض الأوعية الدموية الدماغية
الصغري وتم تشخيصهم إكلينيكيا وباستخدام أشعة الرنين المغناطيسي على تدريج فازيكا.
المجموعة الثانية (المجموعة
الضابطة): اشتملت على عشرة أشخاص غير مصابين بمرض الأوعية الدموية الدماغية الصغري
(نتائج أشعة الرنين المغناطيسي طبيعية).
وقد اجري لهؤلاء الأشخاص
ما يلي: فحص إكلينيكي للجهاز العصبي، الأبحاث المعملية الروتينية، أشعة الرنين
المغناطيسي على المخ، اختبارات نفسية عصبية لقياس الوظائف المعرفية، رسم قلب
وموجات صوتية على القلب.
وقد أظهرت نتائج البحث
وجود فروق ذات دلالة إحصائية بين المجموعة الأولي والمجموعة الثانية في الاختبارات
المعرفية التالية: (اختبار ادينبروك لقياس القدرات المعرفية العامة وبالأخص في الأجزاء
المتعلقة بالذاكرة واللغة والوظائف التنفيذية، وأيضا في الاختبارات المعرفية
الخاصة مثل اختبار رسوم المكعبات لقياس وظيفة الحس الفراغي، اختبار الأزواج
المترابطة لقياس الذاكرة اللفظية، اختبار الطلاقة اللفظية، اختبار الرموز لقياس
المهارات اللغوية واختبارات صنع الدرب والرموز والأرقام لقياس الوظائف التنفيذية).
ويوصي البحث بضرورة الاهتمام بقياسات الوظائف الذهنية والمعرفية لدي الأشخاص