INTRODUCTION
There is growing
evidence that the motor dysfunction in patients with PD is just the tip of the
iceberg of the multifaceted and complex disorder. Cognitive impairment (CI) was
considered one of the most common non-motor complications in PD and it was
found to be associated with significant disability for patients and burden for
caregivers. Similar to motor symptoms, the characteristics of CI in PD can be
quite variable, both in terms of impaired cognitive domains, and the timing of
onset and rate of progression. Cognitive impairment in PD includes mild
cognitive impairment (MCI), cognitive
impairment (CIND) and Parkinson’s disease dementia (PDD)1
Cognitive impairment in PD was found to be present
even in the earlier stages of PD but it may be asymptomatic and can be detected
only with specific neuropsychological tests2 Mild deterioration of such subclinical cognitive dysfunction can
occur in 20-25% of PD patients producing MCI3. Four-year follow-up
study of PD- MCI
patients found that 62% of these patients progressed to PDD. The cognitive
changes in PD are characterized by a frontal-subcortical impairment in which
there is decreased attention and executive function with associated impairment
in visuospatial skills and memory4.
The microstructural
changes in cerebral grey and white matter were recently explored as being one
of the radiological biomarkers for cognitive impairment in PD. DTI plays an
important role in highlighting the cerebral microstructural damage not visible
when conventional sequences are used.
The most DTI metrics used are mean diffusivity (MD), which measures
overall water motion without any directionality, and fractional anisotropy
(FA), which reflects the prevalence of diffusivity along one direction. MD is a
quantitative metric of water diffusion; the higher the MD value, the higher the
diffusivity. FA is a scalar value ranging from zero to one that is highest in
compact WM tracts, decreases in the GM, and approaches zero in the CSF.
Anisotropy was correlated to axon density and myelin content, while diffusivity
was correlated mainly to the amount of myelin. However, both MD and FA have
more recently been shown to be affected mainly by myelin content and, to a
lesser extent, by axonal density.5
In a DTI study on
patients with PD it was found that there was decreased FA in the frontal lobe,
including the medial frontal, cingulate (anterior and posterior), and
dorsolateral prefrontal cortex6.
Concerning cognitive
function, it has been found that there was a significant FA reduction in the
frontal, temporal, and occipital white matter in both PD patients with dementia
(PDD) and PD patients with no dementia (PD-ND) compared with controls. In this
study, PDD subjects showed a significant FA reduction in the bilateral
posterior cingulate bundles compared to PD-ND subjects that was correlated with
global cognition and memory function7.
Aim of work
To explore the cognitive profile of
patients with Parkinson's disease and to correlate the
microstructural changes in cerebral grey and white matter assessed by DTI, with the cognitive pattern in PD.
PATIENTS AND
METHODS
Study design and population: This is a case-control study carried out on 40 patients with idiopathic Parkinson’s disease and
20 normal controls matched for age, sex and level of education. They were recruited in the
period between December 2012 and October 2013 from the Neurology Outpatient
Clinics of Kasr El Aini Hospital and Bani-Suef University Hospital.
Inclusion criteria: 40 patients fulfilled the criteria for diagnosis of
idiopathic Parkinson's disease based on British Brain Bank criteria8.
Selected patients had a score ˃ 24
on Mini-mental state examination (MMSE) 9 and had the ability to read,
write and do simple calculations.
Exclusion criteria: Patients with major language disturbance, severe
physical, auditory or visual impairment affecting their ability to complete
testing, patients with secondary or atypical parkinsonism, patients with
evidence of concomitant cerebrovascular stroke temporally related to the onset
of the disease or the cognitive impairment, patients with concomitant medical,
metabolic illness or major psychiatric disorder known to affect cognition,
patients with marked tremor interfering with the imaging session, patients with
MRI brain showing structural lesion and patients with contraindications for MRI
examination.
Methods: The cognitive function of the PD
patients and controls were assessed using Parkinson’s Disease -
Cognitive Rating Scale (PD-CRS) that covers the full spectrum of cognitive
deficits associated with PD including: attention, episodic memory (immediate and delayed recall),
naming, visuospatial abilities (visuoconstructional and visuoperceptual abilities) and executive
function (working memory, action verbal fluency and alternating verbal
fluency).10
Brain
imaging:
Diffusion tensor imaging (DTI): It was performed for
all PD patients included in the study. Technique was performed using a standard
1.5 Tesla unit. A standard head coil was used. Patient position is supine. The
sequences obtained were axial, sagittal and coronal T1W, T2W, FLAIR and
diffusion tensor, which consisted of a single shot, spin-echo echo planar
sequence in 25 encoding directions and a diffusion-weighting factor of 800
s/mm2. Other parameters include T1WI: TR 450, TE 15, matrix 80 x 81, FOV 230
X177, slice thickness 5 mm, T2WI: TR 3612, TE 100, matrix 208 x 127, FOV 230 X
177, slice thickness 5 mm, FLAIR : TR 6000, TE 120, matrix 240 x 111, FOV
230 X 184, slice thickness 5 mm and DTI :
TR 10951, TE 67, matrix 128 x128, FOV 224 X 224 mm, number of
excitations 2, slice thickness: 2.0/00 and flip angle 90 (degrees). All the
diffusion-weighted images were transferred to the workstation supplied by the
manufacturer. Images were post-processed using the Philips software. The maps obtained were FA 2D grey maps, directionally
encoded color FA maps and fused FLAIR/DTI maps. Fractional anisotropy (FA) was
measured (through application of multiple color-coded regions of interest
(ROI)) in the following structures (in the contralateral side to the side of
motor onset): caudate, putamen, globus pallidus (GP), thalamus, substantia
nigra (SN), hippocampus, and prefrontal white matter.
Statistical Methods
The data were coded and entered using: the statistical package for social
science version 15 (SPSS v 15). Student t- test was used for comparison between
means of two groups of quantitative variables. Chi square test was used for
comparison between two groups of categorical data or frequency of events. The
Pearson correlation coefficient (r) was used to describe the degree of relationship between two variables.
The sign of correlation coefficient (+, -) defines the direction of the relationship, either
positive or negative. The probability/significance value (P value) ≥ 0.05 is
not statistically significant
and <0.05 is statistically significant.
RESULTS
The current study
is a case controls study conducted on 40 patients diagnosed as having Parkinson’s
disease and 20 normal healthy controls. The mean age of PD patients was
59.85±7.51 years, while the mean age of controls was 56.35±5.07 years. 60%
(n=24) of the included PD patients were males with a male: female ratio =
1.5:1. Regarding controls, 55% (n=11) were males with a male: female ratio =
1.22:1. The years of education for PD patients ranged from 2-16 years with a
mean value of 7.5 (SD=4.9) years, and the years of education for controls
ranged also from 2-16 years but with a mean value of 9.6(SD=4.86) years (Table
1).
PD patients were
classified also according to PD-MCI diagnostic criteria proposed by Movement
Disorders Society (MDS) Task Force11 into patients having pre
clinical cognitive impairment (pre clinical CI) [n=23 (57.5%)] and patient
having clinical cognitive impairment (MCI) (clinical CI) [n=17 (42.5%)].
The cognitive function of PD patients and controls
were assessed using Parkinson’s disease -
Cognitive Rating Scale (PD-CRS). PD patients were found to have
significantly lower scores than controls in attention, working memory,
immediate recall, delayed recall, naming, visuoperceptual abilities,
visuoconstructional abilities, action verbal fluency, alternating verbal
fluency and the total score (Table 2).
Comparison between PD patients with clinical CI and those with pre
clinical CI revealed that PD patients with clinical CI have significantly lower
scores than those with pre clinical CI in the scores of the examined cognitive
subsets except naming (Table 3). On comparing PD patients with pre clinical CI
to controls, PD patients with pre clinical CI were found to have significantly
lower scores than controls in working memory and action verbal fluency only.
Diffusion tensor imaging was done for all PD patients included in this
study. FA was measured in the contralateral side to the side of onset of the
motor symptoms in the following regions: thalamus [mean=0.395 (SD=0.124)],
globus pallidus [mean=0.375 (SD=0.141)], putamen [mean=0.416 (SD=0.165)],
caudate [mean=0.411 (SD=0.164)], substantia nigra [mean=0.497 (SD=0.194)],
hippocampus [mean=0.318 (SD=0.135)] and prefrontal region [mean=0.462
(SD=0.182)] (Table 4).
PD patients with clinical CI were found to have significantly lower FA
than those with pre clinical CI in thalamus, GP, putamen, caudate and
prefrontal region, but there was no statistically significant difference
between PD patients with clinical CI and those with pre clinical CI in FA in SN
or hippocampus (Table 5).
There was a statistically significant positive correlation between FA
in thalamus and the scores of attention, working memory, immediate recall,
delayed recall, alternating verbal fluency, action verbal fluency and the total
score. There was a statistically significant positive correlation between FA in
GP and the scores of attention, working memory, alternating verbal fluency,
action verbal fluency and the total score. On the other hand, there was a
statistically significant positive correlation between FA in putamen and the
scores of immediate recall and delayed recall only. There was a statistically
significant positive correlation between FA in caudate and the scores of
attention, working memory, alternating verbal fluency, action verbal fluency
and the total score. Lastly, there was no statistically significant correlation
between FA in SN and the scores of the examined cognitive subsets (Table 6).
There was a statistically significant positive
correlation between FA in hippocampus and the scores of immediate recall and
delayed recall only. On the other hand, There was a statistically significant
positive correlation between FA in prefrontal region and the scores of
attention, working memory, immediate recall, delayed recall, alternating verbal
fluency, action verbal fluency and the total score, but there was no statistically
significant correlation between FA in prefrontal region and the scores of
naming, visuoperceptual abilities, or visuoconstructional abilities (Table 7).
DISCUSSION
Several studies worldwide
have thoroughly investigated the cognitive impairment in PD. The exact pattern
of this impairment and its frequency is still a subject of considerable
controversy. However, most of the cognitive changes in PD are characterized by
a frontal-subcortical impairment in which there is decreased attention and
executive function with associated impairment in visuospatial skills and memory4
On comparing
patients with pre clinical CI to control, patients with pre clinical CI were
found to have significantly lower scores than control in working memory and
action verbal fluency only, and this denotes that executive functions are the
earliest cognitive domains to be affected in patients with PD.
Our finding replicates previous results that point towards both
verbal fluency and working memory tasks as the most sensitive tasks to detect
mild cognitive defects in PDND12. Additionally, action VF was found
to be an early indicator of the conversion from PD-ND to PDD13.
With the
development of neuroimaging techniques such as PET and fMRI, it has been proved
that the deficits in executive functions (measured by verbal fluency and
working memory tasks) observed in the initial phase of PD, depend partly on the
reduction in activity of frontostriatal circuits that connect the basal
ganglions with the prefrontal dorsolateral cortex12.
In the present
study, on comparing patients with clinical CI to control, patients with
clinical CI were found to have significantly lower scores than control not only
in executive function, but also in attention, episodic memory, naming and
visuospatial abilities.
The present results
agreed with the results of several studies who addressed attention, memory,
visuospatial and executive functions as being the most affected cognitive
domains in patients with PD4
Regarding the reported affection of
naming in PD patients in our studies, some studies agreed with our findings and
attributed this affection to the presence of cortical pathology which is
considered a risk factor for developing PDD14.
In the present
study, correlative results revealed that decreased FA in thalamus was
associated with impairment in attention, executive function and episodic
memory. Comparative results revealed that PD patients with clinical CI had
significantly lower FA values than those with pre clinical CI in thalamus.
Our findings agreed
with those obtained by Van Der Werf et al. (1999) and Van Der Werf et al. (2003). The investigators found that severe
deficits in executive functioning and attention were noted in patients with
thalamic lesions15,16. Such reported relationship between thalamus
and, attention, executive function & memory can be attributed to the
presence of functional connectivity between thalamus and hippocampus &
prefrontal cortex17.
In the present
study, correlative results revealed that decreased FA in globus pallidus was
associated with impairment in attention and executive function. Comparative
results revealed that PD patients with clinical CI had significantly lower FA
values than those with pre clinical CI in globus pallidus.
Our results agreed
with the study led by Trepanier et al. (1998). They concluded that ventral
pallidotomy for the treatment of a patient with PD can cause cognitive
impairments18. Additionally, several studies have suggested that
bilateral pallidal lesions may give rise to deficits on tests of executive
function19
In the present
study, correlative results revealed that decreased FA in putamen was associated
with episodic memory impairment. Comparative results revealed that patients
with clinical CI had significantly lower FA values than those with pre clinical
CI in putamen.
In accordance with
our findings, Ystad et al. (2010) and Sadeh et al. (2011 revealed that the
putamen consistently interacts with the hippocampus during episodic memory
formation20,21. In addition, van Beilen & Leenders (2006)
discovered a relationship between putaminal FDOPA uptake and measures for
executive functioning, memory and fluency in patients with PD22.
In the present study,
correlative results revealed that decreased FA in caudate was associated with
impairment in attention and executive function. Comparative results revealed
that PD patients with clinical CI had significantly lower FA values than those
with pre clinical CI in caudate.
Our findings are
consistent with the study conducted by McMurtray et al.
(2008) on eight patients with caudate infarction. The studied patients
were found to have defective performance on tests of memory and frontal
executive functions23. Similar findings
were obtained by Lewis et al. (2003) who conducted a study using fMRI on
patients with PD while they were performing a working memory task12.
In the present
study, correlative results did not reveal any relationship between FA in
substantia nigra and cognitive functions. Comparative results revealed that
there was no statistically significant difference between PD patients with
clinical CI and those with pre clinical CI in FA in substantia nigra. In
contrast to our findings, McKee et al. (1990) found that patients with
substantia nigra lesions might demonstrate profound deficits in working memory
tasks. This was attributed to the presence of channels in SNpr directed at
prefrontal and inferotemporal areas of cerebral cortex24.
In
the present study, correlative results revealed that decreased FA in
hippocampus was associated with episodic memory impairment. Comparative results
revealed that there was no statistically significant difference between PD
patients with clinical CI and those with pre clinical CI in FA in hippocampus.
Our findings were
consistent with those reported by Carlesimo et al. (2012) who found that memory
impairment in patients with PD without dementia may be predicted by the rate of
microstructural alterations in the hippocampal formation as detected by
diffusion tensor imaging analysis25. Additionally, and
Ibarretxe-Bilbao et al. (2008) found a significant association between smaller
hippocampal volumes and poorer memory performance in PD subjects26.
Hippocampal atrophy has also been observed to a lesser degree, in PD-ND27,
and PDND-MCI28, with a greater degree of atrophy being associated
with worse performance on tests of memory function29.
In the present
study, correlative results revealed that decreased prefrontal FA was associated
with impairment in attention, executive function, and episodic memory.
Comparative results revealed that PD patients with clinical CI had
significantly lower prefrontal FA than those with pre clinical CI.
Similar to our findings regarding cognitive function of prefrontal
region, Stebbins et al. (2001) reported a statistically significant positive
correlation between frontal FA and both processing speed and reasoning
performance (components of executive function)30.
Conclusion
The cognitive impairment in PD is present even in the earlier
stages of PD but it may be asymptomatic and can be detected only with specific
neuropsychological tests The cognitive impairment in PD patients can be
attributed to the microstructural changes (decreased FA) in BG, thalamus, hippocampus
and prefrontal white matter.
[Disclosure: Authors report no conflict
of interest]
REFERENCES
1. Langston J. The Parkinson's complex: Parkinsonism is just the tip of the iceberg. Ann Neurol.
2006; 59 (4): 591-6.
2.
Foltynie T, Goldberg T, Lewis S, Blackwell A, Kolachana
B, Weinberger D, et al. Planning ability in Parkinson’s disease is influenced
by the COMT val158met polymorphism. Mov Disord. 2004; 19: 885– 91.
3. Aarsland D, Bronnick K, Williams-Gray C, Weintraub D, Marder K, Kulisevsky J, et al. Mild
cognitive
impairment in Parkinson disease: a multicenter pooled analysis. Neurology. 2010; 75 (12): 1062-9.
4.
Muslimovic D, Post B, Speelman J, Schmand B. Cognitive
profile of patients with newly diagnosed Parkinson disease. Neurology. 2005; 65
(8): 39-1245.
5.
Sullivan E, Pfefferbaum A. Diffusion tensor imaging and
aging. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2006; 30: 749–61.
6.
Karagulle Kendi
A, Lehericy S, Luciana M, Ugurbil K,
Tuite P. Altered diffusion in the frontal lobe in Parkinson disease.
AJNR Am J Neuroradiol. 2008; 29: 501–5.
7.
Matsui H, Nishinaka K, Oda M, Niikawa H, Kubori T,
Udaka F. Dementia in Parkinson's disease: diffusion tensor imaging. Acta Neurol
Scand. 2007; 116 (3): 177-81.
8.
Hughes A, Ben-Shlomo Y, Daniel S, Lees A.
What features improve the accuracy of clinical diagnosis in Parkinson's
disease: a clinicopathologic study. Neurology. 1992; 42: 1142-6.
9.
Folstein M, Folstein S, McHugh P. "Mini-mental
state". A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for
the clinician. J Psychiatr Res. 1975; 12: 189-98.
10. Pagonabarraga J,
Kulisevsky J, Llebaria G, García-Sánchez C, Pascual-Sedano B, Gironell A.
Parkinson's disease-cognitive rating scale: a new cognitive scale specific for
Parkinson's disease. Mov Disord. 2008; 23 (7): 998-1005.
11. Litvan I, Goldman
J, Tröster A, Schmand B, Weintraub D, Petersen R, et al. Diagnostic criteria
for mild cognitive impairment in Parkinson's disease: Movement Disorder Society
Task Force guidelines. Mov Disord. 2012; 27 (3): 349-56.
12. Lewis S, Dove A,
Robbins T, Barker R, Owen A. Cognitive impairments in early Parkinson’s disease
are accompanied by reductions in activity in frontostriatal neural circuitry.
J. Neurosci. 2003; 23: 6351–6.
13. Piatt A, Fields
J, Paolo A, Koller W, Tröster A.
Lexical, semantic, and action verbal fluency in Parkinson's disease with and
without dementia. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 1999; 21(4): 435-43.
14. Pahwa R, Paolo A,
Troster A, Koller W. Cognitive
impairment in Parkinson's disease. Eur J Neurol. 1998; 5 (5): 431-41.
15. Van Der Werf Y,
Weerts J, Jolles J, Witter M, Lindeboom J, Scheltens P. Neuropsychological
correlates of a right unilateral lacunar thalamic infarction. J Neurol
Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1999; 66: 36–42.
16. Van der Werf Y,
Jolles J, Witter M, Uylings H.
Contributions of thalamic nuclei to declarative memory functioning. Cortex.
2003; 39 (4-5): 1047-62.
17. Stein T, Moritz
C, Quigley M, Cordes D, Haughton V, Meyerand E. Functional connectivity in the
thalamus and hippocampus studied with functional MR imaging. AJNR Am J Neuroradiol. 2000; 21 (8): 1397-401.
18. Trepanier L,
Saint-Cyr J, Lozano A, Lang A.
Neuropsychological consequences of posteroventral pallidotomy for the treatment
of Parkinson's disease. Neurology. 1998; 51: 207–15.
19. Haaxma R, van Boxtel A, Brouwer W, Goeken L, Denier van der Gon J, Colebatch J, et al. Motor
function in a patient with bilateral lesions of the globus pallidus. Mov Disord. 1995; 10 (6): 761-77.
20. Ystad M, Eichele
T, Lundervold A, Lundervold A. Subcortical functional connectivity and verbal
episodic memory in healthy elderly--a resting state fMRI study. Neuroimage.
2010; 52 (1): 379-88.
21. Sadeh T, Shohamy
D, Levy D, Reggev N, Maril A.
Cooperation between the hippocampus and the striatum during episodic encoding.
J Cogn Neurosci. 2011; 23 (7): 1597-608.
22. Van Beilen M,
Leenders K. Putamen FDOPA uptake and its relationship to cognitive functioning
in PD. J Neurol Sci. 2006; 248 (1-2): 68-71.
23. McMurtray A,
Sultzer D, Monserratt L, Yeo T, Mendez M. Content-specific delusions from right
caudate lacunar stroke: association with prefrontal hypometabolism. J
Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2008; 20 (1): 62-67.
24. McKee A, Levine
D, Kowall N, Richardson E. Peduncular hallucinosis associated with isolated
infarction of the substantia nigra pars reticulata. Ann Neurol. 1990; 27:
500–4.
25. Carlesimo G, Piras F, Assogna F, Pontieri F, Caltagirone C, Spalletta G. Hippocampal abnormalities
and memory deficits in Parkinson disease: a multimodal imaging study. Neurology. 2012; 78 (24):
1939-45.
26. Ibarretxe-Bilbao
N, Ramírez-Ruiz B, Tolosa E, Martí M, Valldeoriola F, Bargalló N, et al.
Hippocampal head atrophy predominance in Parkinson's disease with
hallucinations and with dementia. J Neurol. 2008; 255 (9): 1324-31.
27. Tam C, Burton E,
McKeith I, Burn D, O'Brien J. Temporal lobe atrophy on MRI in Parkinson disease
with dementia: a comparison with Alzheimer disease and dementia with Lewy
bodies. Neurology. 2005; 64 (5): 861-5.
28. Meyer J, Huang J,
Chowdhury M. MRI confirms mild cognitive impairments prodromal for Alzheimer's,
vascular and Parkinson-Lewy body dementias. J Neurol Sci. 2007; 257 (1-2):
97-104.
29. Nagano-Saito A,
Washimi Y, Arahata Y, Kachi T, Lerch J, Evans AC, et al. Cerebral atrophy and
its relation to cognitive impairment in Parkinson disease. Neurology. 2005; 64
(2): 224-9.
30. Stebbins G,
Poldrack R, Klingberg T, Carillo M, Desmond J, Moseley M, et al. Aging effects
on white matter integrity and processing speed: A diffusion tensor imaging
study. Neurology. 2001; 56 (3): A374.