INTRODUCTION
Apraxia is a disorder
affecting the purposeful execution of learned and meaningful skills. Along with
aphasia, agnosia and amnesia, they represent the neuropsychological syndromes
in stroke patients1. A patient with apraxia has difficulty with, or
is not able to perform learned and purposeful activities. These disturbances in
the organization of voluntary actions are not due to primary motor or sensory
impairments. Neither are these difficulties the result of lack of motivation,
attention, memory or comprehension2.
Patients with ideomotor
apraxia (IMA) have inability to correctly perform gesture, pantomimes and
imitations, whereas actual use of tools is less affected. Patients with IMA do
know what to do, but don’t know how3. The idea or plan of the action
is not impaired, but the implementation of the movement plan into proper action
execution is disrupted4.
Ideomotor
apraxia is caused by the disturbance of the relationship between the sites
storing images for movements and the sites executing them. Lesion of
supramarginal gyrus in the dominant parietal lobe, supplementary motor areas or
corpus callosum, results in apraxia5,6.
Spatial performance in
patients with right hemispheric lesions is adversely affected by lesions
occurring anywhere in a relatively wide area while patients with left
hemisphere damage, with relatively severe damage to a well defined area, show
impaired performance on spatial tasks. Thus patients with dominant hemisphere
infarction appear to make a quick recovery from hemiparesis, but fail to regain
a normal pattern of walking and postural control7.
The aim of this study is
to examine the presence of ideomotor apraxia in stroke patients and to
investigate its impact on functional abilities of such patients.
PATIENTS
AND METHODS
Subjects
Sixty
right handed Egyptian stroke patients and fifteen normal healthy subjects
participated in this study. Patients had hemiparesis due to ischemic
cerebrovascular accident (CVA) in the domain of the carotid system, diagnosed
clinically and by computed tomography (CT) or magnetic resonance imaging (MRI)
of the brain. They were recruited from the outpatient clinics of the Faculty of
Physical Therapy, Cairo
University, and Madinet
Nasr hospital for Health Insurance in the period from June 2011 to May 2012.
Patients were assigned into two groups:
§
Group A: included 30 patients with right
hemiparesis. (16 males and 14 females) with mean of age (51.26±3.67 years), evaluated at three months
(A1a) and re-evaluated at 12 months (A1b) of onset of stroke.
§
Group B: included 30 patients with
left hemiparesis. (16 males and 14 females) with mean age of (54.06±3.67 years), evaluated at three months
(B1a) and re-evaluated at 12 months (B1b) of onset of stroke.
Fifteen normal healthy
subjects matched in age and sex, were also included as a control group.
Inclusion
criteria were: age below 60 years, MMSE > 25, mild degree of spasticity
(grade 1 and grade 1 + according to modified Ashwarth scale). The grade of
motor power of the hemiparetic limb > grade 3.
Exclusion criteria were
hemiparesis due to causes other than
vascular insult age above 60 years, MMSE scores 26, moderate or severe grade of
spasticity, grade of motor power < 3, history of previous or recurrent
strokes, cognitive problems, medically or psychologically unstable patients.
Patients were submitted to:
Thorough
clinical assessment including complete medical and neurological examination.
a.
Mini Mental
State Examination (MMSE): Only
patients with scores more than 25 are included8.
b.
Ideomotor apraxia test: This
test was used to detect ideomotor apraxia in our stoke patients. The IAT
comprises 20 items, divided into 4 categories (facial, upper extremity,
instrumental and complex) each containing 5 items. A correct performance was
awarded 3 points, insufficient but recognizable performance 2 points, and
partially recognizable movements 1 point. When the subject was able to perform
correctly only when given an object, 1 point was awarded. Zero points were
given when an unrecognizable, irrelevant or no response was obtained, or when a
mistake was made in using any object. The maximum score for the test is 60. A score from 57-60 is
considered normal, a score from 51-56 is considered boarder line apraxia, and a
score below 51 is considered apraxia9.
c.
Barthel Index:
Barthel Index (BI) consists of 10 items that measure a person’s daily function
specifically the activities of daily living and mobility. The assessment was
used to determine a baseline level of functioning. The person receives a score
based on whether he or she has received help while doing the task. The scores
for each of the items are summed to create a total sore. The higher the score,
the more independent the person is10.
Statistical
Methods
Data were collected and
statistically analyzed using descriptive statistical analysis. The mean value
as an average describing the central tendency of the observations, the standard
deviation (SD) as a measure of the dispersion of the result around the mean,
independent T-test was used to compare the measurement outcome between two
groups, one way analysis of variance (ANOVA) to compare the measurement outcome
between more than two groups and post hoc to compare each subgroups with each
other and with normal controls.
Pearson correlation (r)
to study correlation between the IAT scores and BI scores, and correlation of
there scores with age and sex of patients.
The difference between
parameters was considered statistically significant if the probability (p)
value is <0.05, highly significant when (p) value is <0.001 and non
significant when (P) value is >0.0511.
RESULTS
Statistical
significant lower mean score of IAT and BI between groups (A1a, A1b) and
controls (Table 2).
Post-hoc Test revealed
A statistically
significant difference was detected between groups A1a, A1b and control group
(group C) as regards mean scores of IAT and BI. After posthoc: Significant
lower mean score of IAT and BI was found in group A1a compared to groups A1b
and controls (p=0.004, 0.0001, 0.0001 respectively).
A significantly lower
mean score of IAT and BI was detected in group A1b compared to group C
(p=0.002, 0.0001 respectively) (Tables 2 and 3).
Statistical significant
lower mean scores between groups B1a, B1b and controls
Post-hoc test revealed
A statically significant
difference was detected between groups B1a, B1b and control group (group c) as
regards mean scores of IAT and BI. After post –hoc: significantly lower mean
scores of IAT and BI in group B1a compared to groups B1b and group C (p=0.01,
0.03, 0.001 respectively).
No significant
difference between mean scores of IAT in group B1b compared to group C (p=0.74,
0.38 respectively). While statistically significant difference between their
mean scores of BI with lower mean in B1b.
Comparison between groups (A1a) and (B1a):
A
statistically highly significant difference was detected between groups (A1a)
and (B1a) as regards the mean scores of IMAT and B1 being significantly lower
in group (A1a) compared to (B1a) (p=0.0001).
Comparison between groups (A1b) and
group (B1b):
A
statistically highly significant difference was detected between groups (A1b)
and (B1b) as regards the mean scores of IAT and B1 significantly lower in group
(A1b) (p=0.0001).
Correlations:
No significant
correlation was found between age of right or left hemiparetic patients with
the scores of IAT, (P = 0.51 and 0.64 respectively) (Figure 2).
No significant
correlation was found between age of right or left hemiparetic patients and
scores of BI test (P= 0.68, 0.78 respectively) (Figure 3).
No significant correlation was detected between
sex and IAT scores in the patients groups A and B (P = 0.27 and 0.22
respectively). (Fig. 4). Moreover, no
significant correlation was detected between sex and BI scores in groups A and
B (p=0.07, 0.14 respectively).
No significant
correlation was found between sex and BI scores in the patients groups (A and
B) (P = 0.07 and 0.22 respectively) (Table 6 and Figure 5).
DISCUSSION
It has been shown that
specific areas of neuropsychological impairment are predictive of functioning
and independence at discharge from acute stroke rehabilitation (12).
Apraxia and pathological emotional reactions were found to be the most
important predictors of dependency in left-hemisphere and right hemisphere
stroke patients respectively13.
In present study,
ideomotor apraxia was found in 14 patients (93.3%) with left hemispheric stroke
evaluated at 3 and 12 months of stoke onset. On the other hand in right
hemispheric stroke patients ideomotor apraxia was detected in: 4 patients
(26.6%) evaluated at 3 months of stoke onset and one patient (6.7%) evaluated
at 12 months of stroke onset. Moreover, in right hemispheric stroke patients,
border line apraxia was found in 3 patients (20%) evaluated at 3 and 12 months
of stroke onset. These results agreed with De Rienzi (1989); Donkervoot et al. (2000),
who found that ideomotor apraxia is a common consequence of left hemisphere
damage
Owing to the dominance
of the left hemisphere on motor control planning.
In
the present study, the mean IMA scores were significantly lower in left
hemispheric stroke patients compared to right hemispheric stroke patients and
control subjects, however the mean IAT scores of right hemispheric stroke
patients were significantly lower compared to control only for those assessed
at 3 months of stroke onset. These finding showed that ideomotor apraxia
disorder is persistent and enduring in left hemispheric stroke patients even
after 12 month of stroke onset, while improvement and even recovery can occur
after 12 months for right hemispheric stroke patients. Our results supported
the findings of Donkervoort et al.14, who reported that about 88% of
left hemispheric stroke the patients were still apractic after 20 weeks which
negatively influenced ADL functioning. Moreover, Donkervoort et al.15,
concluded that apraxia is a persistent disorder and may not recover
spontaneously especially in left hemispheric stroke patients.
Abnormality in imitation
of gestures was detected in 20% of right hemispheric stroke patients and 50% of
left hemispheric patients. These findings agreed with Sunderland
et al.16, who found impaired gesture imitation in 66% of left
hemispheric stroke cases while a marginal impairment was seen in 13% of right
hemispheric stroke. This solid evidence of a high incidence of IMA after left
hemisphere damage gives impetus to theoretical and clinical researchers that we
are not dealing with some rare or non-specific disorder but a common selective
deficit whose nature and clinical implications need to be fully understood.
Using a qualitative
error system, we found that differential patterns of recovery for intransitive
gestures (i.e. gestures not requiring a tool or object, such as "waving
goodbye") and transitive gestures (i.e. gestures requiring tool use),as
there is a decrease in the number of content errors for intransitive gestures,
While spatial and temporal errors were persistent for transitive gestures. Our
result agreed with Raymer et al.4, who found that for intransitive
gestures there was a spontaneous decrease in the number of content errors,
whereas for transitive gestures there was a spontaneous decrease in the number
of unidentifiable production errors. But spatial and temporal errors were found
to be persistent. They concluded that in the natural course of recovery from
apraxia, if performance improves, it is in the areas of meaning and recognition
of gestures.
The functional
assessment of stroke patients is critical for treatment- rehabilitation and
management planning. One of the major goals of treatment-rehabilitation is to
allow patients to live independently. To be independent, persons must be able
to care for themselves and perform ADL that require learned skilled motor
interactions with the environment17,18.
Apractic patients
displayed significant dependency in bathing, toileting and grooming especially
in left hemispheric stroke patients. However, this study failed to establish a
relationship between apraxia and dependence in either ambulation or dressing
which are two major rehabilitation targets for stroke patients. Our results
were in agreement with Kimura19, who found that stroke patients with
IMA were able to dress independently if they had sufficient power in their
paretic limb. Baum and Hall20, also suggested that
inability to dress independently is related to perceptual deficits instead of
deficits in skilled movements.
In the present study,
dependency in feeding was not reported by caregivers of patients with IMA a
finding which agreed with Foundas et al.21.
It
is not clear if specific effects of IMA can be observed in the case of more
complex naturalistic tasks. Goldenberg et al.22, studied V/ tasks
such as changing the batteries in a tape recorder. They found that left
hemi-paretic patients with IMA exhibited more difficulties in performing the
task than patients without apraxia, who in turn were impaired compared to
controls. They suggested that IMA may act as an additional factor, augmenting
the difficulties caused by left hemisphere damage alone, but acknowledged the
alternative possibility that the greater deficit in the apractic cases might
reflect a non-specific depletion of cognition and concluded that such complex
naturalistic tasks may display "cognitive opacity". In other words,
the complexity of the interaction between actor and environment may be such
that we are unable to detect the impact of specific neuropsychological
deficits] A similar conclusion was reached by Stewart et al.11, who
studied tasks such as making a packed lunch and found a similar pattern of
errors in normal controls and in those with right or left hemisphere brain
damage.
On the contrary,
Goldenberg and Hagmann23, studied aphasic patients as they carried
out 3 ADL tasks - buttering bread, donning and doffing a T-shirt, and cleaning
teeth. Only 25% of the sample were able to complete the tasks without error,
and 17% were unable to complete any task. The total frequency of errors
(content and accuracy combined) correlated significantly with tests of
imitation or pantomime but was only weakly related to severity of aphasia,
suggesting a specific impact of IMA.
Walker
et al.24, came to similar conclusions regarding the impact of IMA on
basic ADL tasks — when a motor task such as dressing can be completed using an
over-learned strategy then the apractic patient may be successful, whereas if
hemiparesis demands discovery of a new compensatory strategy then IMA will
present a barrier. This interaction between hemiparesis and apraxia may explain
why IMA has not emerged as a simple correlate of dressing ability in other
studies25,26.
For a long time,
researchers believed that apraxia only occurred when performance was requested
in testing conditions, and that patients would act correctly when performing
spontaneously in a natural context (27, 28). This would suggest that
there is no negative influence of IMA on daily life activities. However, Sundet
et al.(13) found that apraxia variables were significant predictors
of subsequent dependency.
The present work
provides a preliminary evidence of the adverse effect of IMA on activities of
daily living of stroke patients. The demonstration that IMA might be a major
source of disability suggests that its rehabilitation might be an important
part of post-stroke therapy. Consequently, management of IMA should be taking
into consideration in post-stroke rehabilitation in order to improve the
functional outcome of stroke patient
In conclusion ideomotor
apraxia is a common consequence of ischemic CVS especially those affecting the
left cerebral hemisphere. It might be a major cause of disability as it has
adverse impact on activities of daily living of stroke patients. Consequently,
IMA should be taken into consideration when planning rehabilitation program for
stroke patient in order to improve their functional outcome.
[Disclosure:
Authors report no conflict of interest]
REFERENCES
1. Leiguarda RC, Marsden CD. Limb apraxias:
higher-order disorders of sensorimotor integration. Brain.2000; 123(Pt
5):860-79.
2. Kolb B, Whishaw IQ. The parietal lobes.
In: Kolb B, Whishaw IQ, eds. Fundamentals of human neuropsychology, 4th ed. New
York, WH Freeman and Co; 1996. pp. 265 -85.
3. Wheaton
I, Hallett M. Ideomotor apraxia: a
review. J. Neurol Sci. 2007; 260(1-2): 1-10.
4. Raymer AM, Maher LM, FoundasAL, Heilman
KM, RothiLJ. The significance of body part as tool errors in limb apraxia.
Brain Cogn.1997; 34 (2): 287-92.
5. Alexander
MP, Baker E, Naeser MA, Kaplan E, Palumbo C.
Neuropsychological and neuroanatomical dimensions of ideomotor apraxia.
Brain.1992; 115 pt 1: 87-107.
6. Heilman KM, Maher LM, Greenwald ML, Rothi
LJ. Conceptual apraxia from lateralized
lesions. Neurology.1997; 49(2):457-64.
7. Lezak MD, Howieson DB, Loring DW, Hannay
HJ. Neuropsychological assessment, 4th ed. New York, Oxford University
press; 2004.
8. Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR.
(1975): Mini -Mental state. A practical method for grading the cognitive stat
of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatry Research.1975; I2: 189-98.
9. Kaya K, Unsal-Delialioglu S, Kurt M,
Altinok N, Ozel S. Evaluation of
ideomotor apraxia in patients with stroke, a study of reliability and validity.
J Retail Med.2006; 38(2):108- 12.
10. Van Der
Putten JJ, Hobart
JC, Freeman JA, Thompson AJ. Measuring
the change in disability after inpatient rehabilitation: comparison of the
responsiveness of the Barthel Index and Functional Independence Measure. J
Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry.1999; 66 (4): 480 - 4.
11. Motulsky SL. analysis data with GraphPad
prism: GraphPad Software Inc. San Diego CA; 1999.
12. Stewart K J, Gale S D, Diamond PT. Early
assessment of post - stroke patients entering acute inpatient rehabilitation:
Utility of the WASI and HVLT - R American Journal of physical Medicine and
Rehabilitation.2002; 81(3):223-35.
13. Sundet K, Finset A, Reinvang I.
Neuropsychological predictors in stroke rehabilitation: Journal of Clinical and
Experimental Neuropsychology.1988; 10(4): 363-79.
14. Donkervoort J, StehmannSaris FC, Deelman
BG. Efficacy of strategy training in
left hemisphere stroke patients with apraxia: a randomized clinical trial.
Neuropsychol Rehabil.2001; 11:549-66.
15. Donkervoort M, Dekker J, Deelman B. The course of apraxia and ADL functioning in left
hemisphere stroke patients treated in rehabilitation centres and nursing homes:
Clin Rehabil.2006; 20(12): 1085-93.
16. Sunderland
A, Bowers MP, Sluman SM, Wilcock DJ, Ardron ME. Impaired dexterity of the
ipsilateral hand after stroke and the relationship to cognitive deficit:
Stroke.1999; 30: 949-55.
17. Ochipa C, Rothi LJ, Heilman KM. Conceptual
apraxia in Alzheimer's disease: Brain. 1997; 115(pt 4):1061-71.
18. Singh A, Black SE, Herrmann N, Leibovitch
FS, EbertPL, Lawrence J, et al. Functional and neuroanatomic correlations in
poststroke depression: the Sunnybrook Stroke Study. Stroke. 2000; 31(3):637-44.
19. Kimura D. Acquisition of a motor skill after
left-hemisphere damage: Brain.1977; 100(3): 527-42.
20. Baum B,
Hall KM. Relationship between
constructional praxis and dressing in the head-injured adult: Am J Occup
Ther.1981; 35(7):438-42.
21. Foundas
AL, Macauley BL,
Raymer AM, Maher LM, Heilman KM,
Gonzalez Rothi LJ. Ecological
implications of limb apraxia: evidence from mealtime behavior. J Int
Neuropsychol Soc.1995; 1(1):62-6.
22. Goldenberg
G, Daumuller M
, Hagmann S.
Assessment and therapy of complex activities of daily living in apraxia:
Neuropsychological Rehabilitation.2001; 11: 147-69.
23. Goldenberg G, Hagmann S. Tool use and mechanical problem solving in apraxia:
Neuropsychologia.1998; 36(7): 581-9.
24. Walker
CM, Sunderland A, S
Walker MF. The impact of cognitive impairment on upper body
dressing difficulties after stroke: A video analysis of patterns of recovery.
Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry.2004; 75: 43-8.
25. Walker
MF, Lincoln NB.
Factors influencing dressing performance after stroke: J Neurol
Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1991; 54(8): 699-701.
26. Hanna-Pladdy B, Heilman KM, Foundas AL.
Ecological implications of ideomotor apraxia:
Evidence from physical activities of daily living. Neurology.2003;
60(3): 487-90.
27. De Renzi E, Motti F, Nichelli P. Imitating gestures: A quantitative approach
to ideomotor apraxia. Arch Neurol. 1980;
37(1): 6-10.
28. Geschwind
N, Damasio AR. Apraxia. In: Frederiks JAM, editor. Handbook of clinical
neurology. 1985.