INTRODUCTION
Besides the
cerebral cortex, current knowledge on migraine pathophysiology assigns an
important role to both peripheral and central portions of the
trigemino-vascular system. It is generally accepted that the migraine headache
is associated with activation of the latter system1. Researchers
have tried to verify if the same lack of cortical habituation may exist also at
the subcortical trigeminal level.
Migraine
attacks are characterized by a particular sensitivity to visual and auditory
external stimuli2. Some studies suggested that hypersensitivity to
external stimuli may persist between
migraine attacks3. Habituation is a well – known physiological
process which consists of a decreased responsiveness by repetition of the same
stimuli and which may reflect an adaptive mechanism to protect against sensory
over stimulation. For each sensation modality, like as for nociception, there
is an interictal lack of habituation4. This habituation deficit
concerns not only with the cortical
responses but also some brainstem reflexes such as the nociceptive blink reflex
(nBR)5.
Studying
trigeminal reflexes is a useful method for investigating the functional state
of the trigeminal brain stem complex6. The blink reflex (BR), in
particular, has been shown to be highly sensitive to changes in trigeminal
activity. It was found that migraineurs have an interictal habituation deficit
of the BR7. The nBR is elicited by a special stimulation electrode
with high current density activating rather selectively Aδ fibers and has only
a R2 component8,9. While the classical blink R2 reflex habituates
normally in migraine patients10, habituation of the nBR is reduced
interictally11.
The
inherited susceptibility and environmental factors may be interrelated in
migraine12. Genetic load can be seen as determining, on the one
hand, a critical threshold, and on the other hand, it may be responsible for
interictal nervous system dysfunction.
The
habituation deficit of cortical evoked potentials may have a familial character
and was proposed as an endophenotypic marker of migraine13,14.
The aim of this study was to search for an abnormal
habituation pattern of the nBR in healthy asymptomatic subjects who have a
first degree relative affected by migraine, and to compare them with healthy
volunteers and migraine patients.
PATIENTS AND
METHODS
Patients
The study was done in the department of
neurology, Minoufiya University Hospitals. It was approved by the local medical
ethics committee and a written informed consent was taken from all subjects. It
was done on 25 patients suffering from migraine without aura (MO: mean age:
23.8 years; 17 women, 8 men) according to Headache Classification Subcommittee
of the International Headache Society, 20042.
Exclusion criteria for patients included:
1-
Presence of any other pathology explaining
the headache (either diagnosed clinically or by further investigations in the
doubtful cases).
2-
Intake of prophylactic or abortive drugs
for migraine on a regular basis.
3-
Intake of caffeine or alcohol containing
beverages less than 4 hours before the recording.
The patients were compared to 52 healthy volunteers without personal history
of migraine or any other recurrent
headache. They were recruited among
relatives of patients consulting our Neurology Clinic. They were separated
in two groups of comparable age and sex distribution: 27 subjects without
family history of migraine (HV: mean age: 24.6 years; 17 women, 10 men) ,
and 25 subjects having at least one first degree relative who
suffers from migraine (HV-F: mean age: 25.8 years; 16 women, 9 men).
Methods
The nociception-specific blink reflex was elicited
according to the methods described by Kaube et al.8 and Katsarava et
al.9. A custom-built planar concentric electrode providing a high current density at low
intensities was used to stimulate the supraorbital region. Recordings in
patients were obtained interictally at least 2 days after the last and before
the next migraine attack. With surface electrodes, we recorded bilaterally,
over orbicularis oculi muscles, 10 blocks of
five rectified EMG responses with
an interblock interval (IBI) of 2 minutes. The 2-minutes IBI was chosen because
it produces the most pronounced habituation in normal subjects9. The
first sweep of each block was excluded from further analysis to avoid
contamination with a startle response.
Individual
pain thresholds were determined with two ascending and descending sequences of
successive current intensities between 0.2 and 2mA in 0.1mA increments. Areas
under the curve (AUC) of the R2 response were analyzed in each sweep off-line
after demeaning, rectification and averaging between 27 and 87 ms5.
We measured the area under the curve (the response area: RA) in µV × ms.15.
Each block was analyzed separately. Mean values for each block were calculated.
The data were normalized as a percentage deviation from the mean value of five
consecutive sweeps.
Habituation
of the nBR R2 was defined as the percentage change of the R2 area between the
1st and the 10th block of recordings. Since the study by Katasarva et al., that
showed reduced nBR habituation interictally in migraineurs, was based on five
responses obtained with short interstimulus intervals (ISIs)16,
we also measured the amplitude change in the five sequential responses recorded
in the 1st block with an ISI of 15-17 seconds.
Statistical Analysis
Results are presented as means ± standard deviations. Group
differences in mean pain threshold and nBR latency were calculated with one-way
analysis of variance (ANOVA). Habituation of the nBR responses was assessed by
ANOVA for repetitive measurements with Scheffé's post hoc analysis, considering
the different blocks (2 to 10) as within subject factors and the diagnostic
groups (HV, MO and HV-F) as between subject factors. Results were considered
significant at P<0.05.
RESULTS
No significant difference was found in mean perception or
pain threshold between the three subject groups on either side of the forehead,
or between the left and right side in any group. So, the mean stimulus
intensities (1.5 × pain threshold) used for studying the nBR were similar
between groups: 2.15±0.51 mA for HV; 2.11±0.72 mA for HV-F and 2.21±0.84 mA for
MO.
Mean R2 latency was slightly shorter in
migraineurs than in both groups of healthy volunteers (MO: 37.62±5.78 ms; HV:
41.81±7.45 ms; HV-F: 39.88±4.61 ms), but these differences were not significant
[F(1,35) = 2.926, P = 0.10 versus HV; F(1,34) = 1.171, P = 0.29 versus HV-F]
(Figure 1).
There was no significant side difference in first block nBR
response area or in its habituation over 10 blocks in any of the three groups (Table 1). The response area in the first block of stimuli
was greater in healthy controls than in migraineurs or healthy volunteers with an affected first
degree relative, but this difference did not reach statistical significance [F
(1,35) = 1.564, P = 0.22 versus MO; F(1,33) = 0.826, P = 0.39; (Table 1).
Figure 1. Comparison among the groups
regarding the mean R2 latency.
Table 1. R2
response area in the first block of averaged responses (mV_ms).
Subject
group
|
Ipsilateral
|
Contralateral
|
Healthy
subjects
(HV;
n =27)
|
1. 06 ±0.69
|
0.87±0.60
|
Healthy
+1st degree
migraine
(HV-F;
n =25)
|
0.89±0.59
|
0.64±0.42
|
Migraine
without
aura
(MO;
n =25)
|
0.85±0.47
|
0.62±0.42
|
One-way
ANOVA
|
NS
|
NS
|
In HV there was a strong habituation with an amplitude
decrease exceeding 50% between the first and the 10th block of five averagings.
This contrasted with a less than 30% habituation in the MO [F(1,35) = 13.323, P
= 0.001 versus HV] and HV-F [F(1,32) = 4.71, P = 0.049 versus HV] groups (Table 2).
Habituation steadily increased in successive blocks
in all the three groups of subjects up to the 10th block of stimuli. The
difference in the degree of nBR RA habituation between healthy volunteers and
the other two groups, however, was already significant in the second block of
five averagings: 25.2% habituation in HV, –8.9% (i.e. potentiation) in HV-F
[F(1,33) = 7.810, P = 0.010 versus HV] and 1.9% habituation in MO [F(1,35) =
5.151, P = 0.037 versus HV]. Habituation of the R2 response area of ipsi- and contralateral
nBR in 10 blocks of five averagings (interstimulus interval: 15–17 ms; interblock interval: 2 min) expressed as percentage
of the 1st block.
Table 2. Habituation of the R2
response area in the last block of
averaging relative to the 1st block (%).
Subject group
|
Ipsilateral
|
Contralateral
|
Healthy
subjects
(HV;
n =27)
|
55.07±23.49
|
54.08±22.49
|
Healthy
+1st degree
migraine
(HV-F;
n =25 )
|
26.79±45.05
|
27.42±40.82*
|
Migraine
without
aura
(MO;
n =25)
|
25.77±21.33**
|
23.71±24.82**
|
One-way
ANOVA
|
*P = 0.049
**P =0.001
|
*P=
0.044
**P = 0.001
|
A positive intra-individual correlation
was found between attack frequency and habituation in MO (r = 0.598; P =
0.010). Single response areas within the
1st block of stimuli and their habituation in individual subjects greatly varied.
There was nonetheless a significant reduction in habituation between the first
and fifth response in the HV-F group. For instance, on the ipsilateral side the
amplitude change was -37.5% (potentiation) in HV-F, compared to +42.1%
(habituation) in HV [F(1,33) = 4.760; P = 0.04]. Habituation was also lower in
MO (30.5%) than in HV, but this difference was not significant [F(1,35) =
0.811; P = 0.45].
DISCUSSION
Several
studies have searched for different biochemical and neurophysiological
abnormalities that are able to explain
the permanent perpetuation of the migraine attacks, which, if present, should
also be detectable in the pain-free period, representing underlying
dysfunctions. Patients with migraine were observed to show an increased
cortical bioelectrical activity with respect to normal subjects. Early
investigators observed increased amplitudes of visual responses evoked by light
flashes or by pattern-reversal stimulation in migraineurs between attacks.
However, the question arose whether this excessive increase in amplitude could
be due to an insufficient habituation and whether the latter could be a more
general abnormality in processing incoming information in migraineurs brain16.
The
results of this study showed that habituation of the nBR is significantly
reduced interictally in migraineurs compared to healthy volunteers, and this
was in line with those reported by Katsarava et al.11. The
intrablock habituation values were smaller in migraineurs than in healthy
volunteers, but this difference was not significant, possibly because the
standard deviations were too large and groups too small. The difference in the
habituation of nBR response area between patients and healthy volunteers was
significant as early as the 2nd block of averages. It increased with repetition
of stimuli and tended to become maximal in the 10th block, a time at which
habituation was found to be maximal in normal subjects9. Di Clemente
et al. found that the first nBR response had a tendency to be lower in
migraineurs than in healthy subjects. This observation was inconsistent with an
abnormality involving trigeminal nucleus sensitization and rather suggested
that the reflex could be hypoexcitable in-between attacks5,17. The
question of hypo- or hyperexcitability is still debated regarding the pathophysiology
of interictal habituation deficit observed in migraineurs18. The
habituation of the blink reflex is reduced interictally in migraine patients
during short 7,11. as well as long time courses5,17.
Few
correlations have been found between neurophysiological results and frequency
of migraine attacks. Our results showed positive intra-individual correlation
between attack frequency and habituation in MO.
In another study, it was found that in episodic migraine, the
habituation deficit of BR decreased with increasing attack frequency which
suggested that it was unlikely to be due to trigeminal sensitization5,17.
This does not seem to be the case in chronic migraine patients who displayed a
lower degree of blink reflex habituation when studied outside an attack19.
Additionally, de Tommaso et al. studied
laser-evoked cortical responses and proved that the habituation deficit found interictally in migraineurs was
positively correlated with attack frequency 20 . In a
magnetoencephalographic recording of somatosensory evoked potentials, the
amplitude increase in migraineurs was linked to the frequency of attacks 21.
All
these results are in contrast with our finding of a negative correlation
between the habituation deficit on the nBR and attack frequency. It could,
however, be related to the fact that nBR short-term habituation 11,
parallel to habituation of evoked cortical potentials22, normalizes
during the attack period. The patients with high-attack frequencies may be at
greater risk of being recorded in closer vicinity to an attack.
The classical R2 is essentially normal interictally, but it
is less influenced by the warning of the stimulus in migraine 10
and, at short IBI, its habituation was found reduced in migraineurs who
developed an attack within 3 days after the recording7. The rather
low nBR amplitude interictally in migraineurs, contrasts with the 680% nBR RA
increase reported during the migraine attack23. This may reflect
ictal sensitization of spinal trigeminal nucleus neurons24. The low
nBR amplitude did not favor such sensitization and rather suggested that the R2
interneurons and circuit could be hypoexcitable in-between migraine attacks.
Lack of habituation might be a consequence of reduced serotoninergic
transmission25,26. leading to a decreased preactivation level.
The pathophysiology of this habituation deficit may be
related to an altered serotoninergic transmission. The serotoninergic system
plays an important role in the endogenous pain control system and has been
implicated in migraine pathophysiology 25
. Strong intensity dependence of auditory evoked potentials in migraine
habituation pattern tends to normalize with fluoxetine prophylaxis which is a
serotonin re – uptake inhibitor27.
This study showed that asymptomatic subjects with a first
degree migrainous relative gave the same nBR abnormalities as patients with
full-blown migraine between attacks. Compared to healthy volunteers without a
family history of migraine, they tended to have smaller first block nBR
response areas and reduced nBR habituation. Their degree of habituation was
intermediate between that of healthy controls who habituated more and that of
migraineurs who habituated less, but the difference was significant respective to the former, but not
to the latter. Healthy subjects at risk also had a significant reduction of
habituation within the 1st block of five responses, which differed from the
findings in migraineurs.
Some migraine phenotypes appear to be complex genetic
disorders, where additive genetic effects (susceptibility genes) and
environmental factors are interrelated 28. Various gene
polymorphisms may be more prevalent in migraineurs than in controls. Although
certain rare migraine subtypes such as familial hemiplegic migraine are
monogenic diseases, there is increased evidence that the common forms of
migraine are polygenic multifactorial diseases, where the genotype determines a
migraine threshold which is modulated by internal (e.g. hormonal) and
environmental factors28-30.
The
link between genetic determinants and abnormal information processing may exist
in migraine31. One of the migraine interictal electrophysiological
abnormalities is a marked intensity dependence of auditory evoked cortical
potentials (IDAP), which is obtained after stimulations of increasing
intensities (25). The IDAP habituation
was investigated in 20 pairs of migraineurs, i.e. parents and their children.
Children tended to have more abnormal values than parents32.
Another
interictal abnormality tested was the contingent negative variation (CNV). It
is a slow cortical potential related to higher mental functions, used to study
habituation in migraine. Its early component showed that habituation was
markedly decreased or even abolished in patients affected by migraine without aura
between attacks 33.
Siniatchkin et al, observed strong correlations according to the
amplitude and habituation of the early CNV component (iCNV) between children
suffering from migraine and their parents with migraine and between young
migraineurs and their healthy parents who have a positive family history of
migraine 32. Moreover, lack of habituation of the iCNV was found in
asymptomatic subjects with a positive family history of migraine, defined ‘‘at
risk”, and the amplitude of the iCNV correlated significantly with the relative
number of subjects suffering from migraine in the family 34. The same observation was present with
nociceptive blink reflex in a group of healthy asymptomatic subjects having a
1st degree relative affected by migraine 5.
The
previous studies and our results raise the possibility that subjects with a
familial predisposition for migraine may present a presymptomatic
neurophysiological abnormality in response habituation, i.e. the same
habituation deficit as migraine patients. A longitudinal follow-up study may be
conducted for healthy subjects at high risk and to compare the genotypes. This
may help to determine which factors such as personality traits, life events,
environment or co-morbidity can protect subjects with a family history of
migraine against developing migraine.
[Disclosure: Author reports
no conflict of interest]
REFERENCES
1. Burstein R., Yarnitsky D, Goor-Aryeh I,
Ransil BJ, Bajwa ZH. Association between migraine and cutaneous allodynia. Ann
Neurol. 2000; 47: 614-24.
2. Headache classification Committee: The international
classification of headache disorders. Cephalalgia 2004; S1: 88- 101.
3. Mulleners WM, Chronicle EP, Palmer
JE, Koehler PJ,. Vredeveld JW. Visual cortex excitability in migraine with and without aura. Headache.
2001; 41: 565-72.
4. Valeriani M, de Tommaso M, Restuccia D, Le Pera D, Guido M, Iannetti GD, et al.
Reduced habituation to experimental pain in migraine patients. A CO (2) laser
evoked potential study. Pain. 2003; 105: 57-64.
5. Di Clemente L, Coppola G, Magis D, Fumal
A, De Pasqua V, Di Piero V, et al. Interictal habituation deficit of the
nociceptive blink reflex: An endophenotypic marker for presymptomatic migraine?
Brain. 2007; 130, 765-70.
6. Esteban A. A neurophysiological approach
to brainstem reflexes. Blink reflex. Neurophysiologie Clinique. 1999. 29; 7-38.
7. De
Marinis M, Pujia A, Natale L, D'arcangelo E, Accornero N. Decreased habituation
of the R2 component of the blink reflex in migraine patients. Clin
Neurophysiol. 2003; 114: 889-93.
8. Kaube
H, Katsarava Z, Kaufer T, Diener HC, Ellrich J. A new method to increase
nociception specificity of the human blink reflex. Clin Neurophysiol. 2000; 111:
413-6.
9. Katsarava
Z, Ellrich J, Diener HC, Kaube H. Optimized stimulation and recording
parameters of human “nociception specific” blink reflex recordings. Clin
Neurophysiol. 2002a; 113: 1932-6.
10. De
Tommaso M, Murasecco D, Libro G, Guido M, Sciruicchio V, Specchio LM, et
al. Modulation of trigeminal reflex
excitability in migraine: effects of attention and habituation on the blink
reflex. Int J Psychophysiol. 2002;44: 239-49.
11. Katsarava
Z, Giffin N, Diener HC, Kaube H. Abnormal habituation of “nociceptive” blink
reflex in migraine – evidence for increased excitability of trigeminal
nociception. Cephalalgia. 2003; 23: 814-9.
12. Gervil M, Ulrich V, Kaprio J, Olesen J, Russell MG. The relative
role of genetic and environmental factors in migraine without aura. Neurology.
1999; 53: 995-9.
13. Sándor
PS, Áfra J, Proietti-Cecchini A, Albert A, Schoenen J. Familial influences on
cortical evoked potentials in migraine. Neuroreport. 1999; 10: 1235-8.
14. Siniatchkin
M, Kirsch E, Kropp P, Stephani U, Gerber WD. Slow cortical potentials in
migraine families. Cephalalgia. 2000; 20: 881-92.
15. Ellrich
J, Treede RD. Characterization of blink reflex interneurons by activation of
diffuse noxious inhibitory controls in man. Brain Res. 1998; 803: 161-8.
16. Ambrosini A, de Noordhout AM, Sandor PS,
Schoenen J. Electrophysiological studies in migraine: A comprehensive review of
their interest and limitations. Cephalalgia. 2003; 23(Suppl. 1): 13-31.
17. Di Clemente L, Coppola G, Magis D, Fumal A,
De Pasqua V, Schoenen J. Nociceptive blink reflex and visual evoked potential
habituations are correlated in migraine. Headache. 2005; 45: 1388-93.
18. Coppola G, Ambrosini A, Di Clemente L, Magis
D, Fumal A, Gérard P, et al. Interictal abnormalities of gamma band activity in
visual evoked responses in migraine: An indication of thalamocortical dysrhythmia?
Cephalalgia. 2007; 27: 1323-30.
19. De Marinis M, Pujia A,
Colaizzo E, Accornero N. The blink reflex in ‘‘chronic migraine”. Clin
Neurophysiol. 2007; 118: 457-63.
20. De
Tommaso M, Lo Sito L, Di Fruscolo O, Sardaro M, Pia Prudenzano M, Lamberti P, et
al. Lack of habituation of nociceptive evoked responses and pain sensitivity
during migraine attack. Clin Neurophysiol. 2005; 116: 1254-64.
21. Lang
E, Kaltenhauser M, Neundorfer B, Seidler S. Hyperexcitability of the primary
somatosensory cortex in migraine – a magnetoencephalographic study. Brain.
2004; 127: 2459-69.
22. Schoenen J, Ambrosini A, Sandor PS, De Noordhout A. Evoked
potentials and transcranial magnetic stimulation in migraine: published data
and viewpoint on their pathophysiologic significance. Clin Neurophysiol. 2003; 114:
955-72.
23. Kaube
H, Katsarava Z, Prywara S, Drepper J, Ellrich J, Diener HC. Acute migraine
headache. Possible sensitization of neurons in the spinal trigeminal nucleus?
Neurology. 2002; 58: 1234-8.
24. Katsarava
Z, Lehnerdt G, Duda B, Ellrich J, Diener HC, Kaube H. Sensitization of
trigeminal nociception specific for migraine but not pain of sinusitis.
Neurology 2002b; 59: 1450-3.
25. Wang
W, Timsit-Berthier M, Schoenen J. Intensity dependence of auditory evoked
potentials is pronounced in migraine: an indication of cortical potentiation
and low serotonergic neurotransmission? Neurology. 1996; 46: 1404-9.
26. Juckel
G, Molnar M, Hegerl U, Csepe V, Karmos G. Auditory evoked potentials as
indicator of brain serotonergic activity – first evidence in behaving cats.
Biol Psychiatry. 1997; 41: 1181-95.
27. Ozkul
Y, Bozlar S. Effects of fluoxetine on habituation of pattern reversal visually
evoked potentials in migraine prophylaxis. Headache. 2002; 42: 282-7.
28. Montagna
P. Molecular genetics of migraine headaches: a review. Cephalalgia. 2000; 20: 3-14.
29. Sandor PS, Ambrosini A, Agosti RM, Schoenen J. Genetics of
migraine: Possible links to neurophysiological abnormalities. Headache. 2002;
42: 365-77.
30. Haan
J, Kors EE, Vanmolkot KR, van den Maagdenberg AM, Frants RR, Ferrari MD.
Migraine genetics: an update. Curr Pain Headache Rep. 2005; 9: 213-20.
31. Sandor PS, Afra J,
Proietti-Cecchini A, Albert A, Schoenen J. Familial influences on cortical
evoked potentials in migraine. Neuroreport. 1999; 10: 1235-8.
32. Siniatchkin M, Kirsch E, Kropp P, Stephani U, Gerber W. D. Slow
cortical potentials in migraine families. Cephalalgia. 2000c; 20: 881-92.
33. Kropp P, Gerber WD.
Contingent negative variation during migraine attack and interval: Evidence for
normalization of slow cortical potentials during the attack. Cephalalgia. 1995;
15(2): 123-8.
34. Siniatchkin M, Kropp P, Gerber WD.
Contingent negative variation in subjects at risk for migraine without aura.
Pain. 2001; 94(2): 159-67.