INTRODUCTION
Psychotic
major depression is a discrete disorder and separate from non-psychotic major
depression1,2. They differ in presenting features, biological
measures, familial transmission, course and outcome, and response to treatment3-5.
Mood disorders may be associated with a distinct pattern of cognitive impairment6-8.
About 50% to 75% of all depressed patients have a cognitive impairment,
sometimes referred to as depressive pseudo-dementia9-11. Persons who
are depressed may have (reversible) cognitive deficits as a consequence of
motivational and attention problems12,13. Cognitive impairment is
also likely to be a key factor affecting the subject's ability to function
occupationally and, hence, the timing of his or her return to work14-17.
It has been proposed that raised cortisol levels during depression might be
associated with cognitive impairments18-20. Changes in hippocampal activity may be also
involved in steroid-related cognitive changes21,22. It was
hypothesized that corticosteroids act to suppress the ability of the
hippocampus to filter out behaviorally irrelevant stimuli.23,24,26-29.
The aim
of this work is to find out the different cognitive deficits in major depressed
patients psychotic and non-psychotic and the relation of these deficits to the
levels of cortisol.
PATIENTS AND METHODS
Patients
This comparative cross-sectional study was conducted on 80
patients selected from the psychiatric outpatient clinic of Kasr Al-Aini
Hospital. They were
divided into two groups: Group one: Consisted of 40 patients diagnosed as major
depressive disorder with psychotic features. Group two: Consisted of 40
patients diagnosed as major depressive disorder without psychotic features.
They were diagnosed according to the fourth edition of Diagnostic and
Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV-TR), Axis I disorders30,
by the residents and were reassessed by a senior psychiatrist to confirm
diagnosis. Inclusion criteria: Age
ranged between 20 and 50 years, both sexes were included and patients should be
un-medicated or with a washout period of antipsychotics and/or antidepressants
for at least 2 weeks. Exclusion criteria: Patients with major medical illnesses particularly
endocrinopathies, patients taking hormonal therapy, patients with history of
neurological disorders or of substance dependence or abuse within 6 months,
history of head injury with loss of consciousness, and other medical disorders
that may affect brain functions, or recent/current ECT or treatment with
anticonvulsants or benzodiazepines. This was to avoid any factors that might
affect cognitive functions or hormonal profile of our patients.
Methods
Informed
consent and a full clinical psychiatric sheet were taken for each patient. All
patients were subjected to the following:
Laboratory
Investigations
Estimation of cortisol level at 8.00 am and at 8.00 pm in the same day for each individual using
Radioimmune Assay. A large number of cortisol immunoassays have been developed
using different types of antibodies, processing schemes, labels, and
measurement principles including beta and gamma counting, photometry,
fluorometry, and chemiluminescence31. Cortisol is quantitatively
displaced from endogenous binding proteins by protein-binding agents such as
8-anilino-1-naphthalene-sulfonic acid (ANS) or salicylate, by low pH, or by
heat treatment32.
Hamilton Rating Scale for
Depression (HRSD)
This
scale contains 24 items to assess the severity of an illness, scored 0 to 4 or
0 to 2 with a total score of 0 to 76. The original recommendation for this
scale was that it is not to be used as a checklist, but with the clinical
judgment of a skilled rater using all sources of information33.
Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised (WAIS-R)
It is
the best standardized and most widely used intelligence test in clinical
practice today. The WAIS comprises 11 subtests made up of six verbal subtests
(Information, Comprehension, Arithmetic, Similarities, Digit span and
Vocabulary) and five performance subtests (Picture completion, Block design,
Picture arrangement, Object assembly and Digit symbol), which yield a verbal
IQ, a performance IQ, and a full-scale IQ. Variability in functioning is
revealed through discrepancies between verbal and performance IQs and by the
scatter pattern between subtests34.
Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST)
It
assesses abstract reasoning and flexibility in problem solving. Stimulus cards
of different color, form, and number are presented to patients for sorting into
groups according to a principle established by the examiner but unknown to the
patient. Successful performance on the WCST requires a complex set of cognitive
functions, including the ability to think abstractly, selectively attend to a
particular perceptual dimension, and shift cognitive set35.
Statistical Methods
The
mean scores on variable x are symbolized by x (read “X bar”) and is computed
with the formula: X= Σxi / N Which instructs one to add all the scores (i.e.,
Σxi) and divide by N, which is the number of scores in the distribution. The
standard deviation (symbolized by S), is defined to be the positive square root
of the variance: S= √Σ (Xi ─ X) ∕ N-1(36). There are two versions of
t- test. The paired-samples t-test (also known as the matched pair’s t-test,
the related t-test, and the correlated t-test) compares the means of two paired
sets of data.(37). Values of the Spearman Correlation Coefficient
range from -1 to +1.36
RESULTS
The two groups showed no differences regarding age, years
of formal education and marital status. They significantly differed regarding
sex distribution (Table 1).
There was non significant difference between the two
groups’ mean on the HRSD nor Total IQ. However they differed significantly
regarding the obtained means on verbal, performance and deterioration index
(Table 2).
The two study groups differed significantly regarding
several domains of the WCST, where the psychotic group was performing more
poorly than the non-psychotic group in this test, table 3.
Regarding
cortisol level, the psychotic group obtained a significantly higher morning
level and a significantly lower evening level. Also the psychotic group
obtained a significantly higher percentage of change over the day (Table 4).
There
was significant negative and positive correlations between Hamilton symptoms and cortisol levels and
percent change of cortisol in psychotic group and non-psychotic group (Table
5).
There
was significant negative and positive correlation correlations between cortisol
levels and WAIS-R subtests, IQ, deterioration index and WCST in psychotic and
non-psychotic group (Table 6).
There
was significant correlation between WAIS-R subtests & Hamilton symptoms in psychotic and non-
psychotic groups (Tables 7 and 8).
By
using Factor Analysis of the Covariance Matrix we found: the loading plot
showed that arithmetic and picture
completion are the most two important and orthogonal (independent) variables that interpret variations in
psychotic group, and that comprehension and block design are the most two
important and orthogonal (independent)
variables that interpret variations in non-psychotic group.
DISCUSSION
In our
study there was a statistically significant difference between the two studied
groups as regard mean of 8:00 am
cortisol, mean of 8:00 pm
cortisol and percent change of cortisol level during a day. Although, the
values of cortisol level at both am and pm in both groups are in the normal
ranges of the reference laboratory (6-16 µg for 8:00 am and 3-11 µg for 8:00
pm), however the psychotic group had a mean value for 8:00 am significantly
higher than that of non-psychotic group. Normally, it is expected to have a
change in the value of 8:00 pm
cortisol level ≤ 50% of the value of 8:00
am cortisol level in a day for a healthy subject38. In
the psychotic group the mean change was more than 50% of 8:00 am value, while non-psychotic group were
in normal range of the percent of change, which may reflect that, the activity
of HPA axis differs in the two groups of the patients. The psychotic group
showed more pronounced disturbance in HPA axis activity. These results are
consistent with the results of Schatzberg et al.39, who found a
statistically significant group differences during three consecutive time
points in the late afternoon (3:00 pm,
3:30 pm and 4:00 pm). In a study carried out by
O’Brien et al.40, there was a statistically significant difference
between depressed subjects and healthy comparison subjects as regard mean of 8:00 am cortisol, mean of 8:00 pm cortisol and percent change
of cortisol level during a day.
Results of this study showed no statistically significant
difference between the two studied groups regarding Hamilton Rating Scale mean
scores. These results are consistent with Nelson et al.41. However
our results are inconsistent with Schatzberg et al.39 as they found
that, Hamilton Rating Scale scores were significantly higher for psychotic
major depression group than for the non-psychotic major depression group.
Similar results were reported by Schatzberg et al.4 and by Hill et
al.2. Comparing our results to other studies, it was found that the
mean score of HRSD for both groups in our study was higher than the
corresponding groups in these studies, which might be explained by different
culture and psychosocial stressors.
On WAIS
the two groups differed significantly regarding their mean scores on
performance, verbal and deterioration index.
Psychotic
patients performed more poorly than non-psychotic patients in both verbal,
performance IQ and deterioration index, which might be attributed to cognitive
changes which are assumed to be more pronounced in psychotic patients whether
due to presence of delusions and hallucinations which could affect their
concentration and performance or due to changes occurring in frontal area or
hippocampus as a part of elevated cortisol level42,43. In a study
done by Schatzberg et al.4 patients with psychotic major depression
performed more poorly than did non-psychotic and comparison groups on the
WAIS-R verbal subtest. In another study done by Schatzberg et al.39,
significant differences were not observed among the three groups (psychotic,
non-psychotic & comparison) on the WAIS verbal test scores.
On the
WCST the performance of psychotic patients was poorer than that of
non-psychotic. The results showed a statistically high significant difference
between both studied groups regarding number of trials, number of categories
completed, total number of correct, total number of errors, percentage of
errors, perservative response and percentage of perservative response. Also, it
was found that there were a statistically high significant difference between
the two groups in perservative errors, percentage of perservative errors,
conceptual level of response, percentage of conceptual level of response and
the number of trials to complete 1st category. The greater deficits
in the patients with psychotic major depression might be due either to
disorder-specific effects on prefrontal cortex function or to general illness
severity. Merriam et al.44 who were studying the prefrontal cortical
dysfunction in depression determined by WCST and found that patients with major
depression exhibited significant deficits when performing the WCST. These
deficits were less severe than those in patients with schizophrenia (they
studied 79 major depressed patients only 5 of them were psychotic and 47
schizophrenic and schizoaffective patients). Their finding of a significant
association between depressive symptoms and the magnitude of WCST deficits in
major depression suggested that there may be a relationship between cognitive
impairments and state-related physiological abnormalities of prefrontal cortex
function during episodes of depression.
Correlation Studies: In
psychotic group: There were positive correlations between level of cortisol at
8:00 am and performance of the patients in subtests of picture completion,
picture arrangement and digit symbol, which means that high levels of cortisol
at 8:00 am were required to perform better in these subtests which are part of
performance domain of WAIS-R test. (N.B. the mean value of cortisol level am
and pm were in normal ranges of our reference laboratory). There were a
positive correlation between level of cortisol at 8:00 pm and performance on digit span and negative
correlation to vocabulary subtest. Both subtests are part of verbal domain of
WAIS-R test. As such, we can suggest that, the performance subtests are related
to levels of cortisol at the morning while verbal subtests are related to
levels of cortisol at evening in psychotic group of patients. There were
positive correlations between a.m. cortisol and symptoms of helplessness,
hopelessness, suicide and guilt which mean that these symptoms became more
severe when levels of am cortisol was high.
In non-psychotic group: The
level of cortisol at a.m. was positively correlated to subtests of verbal
domain and to subtests of performance domain, as the level of a.m. cortisol
increased, the performance on these subtests improved. Regarding WCST, there
were positive and negative correlation
between a.m. cortisol p.m. cortisol and percent of change in cortisol, which
mean that the performance of non-psychotic patients on these tests is decreased
if the change in cortisol levels between morning and evening was high. These
may suggest that, the cognitive decline in these patients might be related to
high discrepancies between levels of cortisol in morning and evening rather
than its high levels purse. There were positive correlation between a.m.
cortisol and symptoms of disturbed work, sense of helplessness and suicide,
while p.m. cortisol was negatively correlated to depressed mood and suspicions.
Percent change of cortisol was negatively correlated to symptoms of depressed
mood, disturbed work, sense of helplessness and suicide as the percent change
increases, the symptoms decreases, which may reflect the role of
non-suppression of cortisol level as aggravating the symptoms of major
depression in both psychotic and non-psychotic patients. Information subtest
was negatively correlated to presence and severity of somatic symptoms.
Comprehension was negatively correlated to presence of obsessions while it was
positively correlated to presence of anxiety. Anxiety itself had positive
correlations to arithmetic, picture arrangement, and block design subtests and
had a negative correlation to vocabulary subtest. This could be explained by
that, moderate levels of anxiety can have an arousing function that serves
behavioral performance up to a point, the point of disability coming when the
anxiety symptoms become so severe as to overwhelm information-processing
resources45.
Factor
analysis and many of the correlative studies, although of limited clinical
relevance, yet it showed that psychotic and non- psychotic depression are
different.
Conclusion
Both
patients with psychotic major depression and non-psychotic major depression had
definite cognitive deficits. Cognitive deficits in depressed patients (both
psychotic and non-psychotic) are not age related. Elevated cortisol level and
percent of change of cortisol levels are related to the severity of depressive
symptoms in both psychotic and non-psychotic depressed patients.
[Disclosure: Authors report
no conflict of interest]
REFERENCES
1. Schatzberg AF, Rothschild AJ. Psychotic
(delusional) major depression: should it be included as a distinct syndrome in
DSM-IV? Am J Psychiatry. 1992; 149: 733-45.
2. Hill
SK, Keshavan MS, Thase ME,
Sweeney JA. Neuropsychological dysfunction in antipsychotic-naïve first episode
unipolar psychotic depression. Am J Psychiatry. 2004; 161: 996-1003.
3. Rothschild AJ, Benes F, Hebben N, Woods B,
Luciana M, Bakanas E, et al. Relationships between brain CT scan findings and
cortisol in psychotic and nonpsychotic depressed patients. Biol Psychiatry.
1989; 26: 565-575.
4. Schatzberg AF, Posener JA, DeBattista C,
Kalehzan M, Rothschild AJ, Shear PK. Neuropsychological deficits in psychotic
versus nonpsychotic major depression and no mental illness. Am J Psychiatry.
2000; 157: 1095-100.
5. Maj M, Pirozzi
R, DeCaprio EL. Major depression with mood-congruent psychotic features: a
distinct diagnostic entity or a more severe subtype of depression? Acta
Psychiatr Scand. 1990; 82: 439-44.
6. MacQueen G, Frodl T. The hippocampus in
major depression: evidence for the convergence of the bench and bedside in
psychiatric research?. Molecular Psychiatry. 2010; (27 July) |
doi:10.1038/mp.2010.80.
7. Desseilles M, Balteau E, Sterpenich V,
Dang-Vu TT, Darsaud A, Vandewalle G et al. Abnormal Neural Filtering of
Irrelevant Visual Information in Depression.
The Journal of Neuroscience. 2009; February 4, 29(5): 1395-1403.
8. Jorm AF. Is depression a risk factor for
dementia or cognitive decline? A review. J Gerontol. 2000; 46: 219-227.
9. Butters MA, Young JB, Lopez O, Aizenstein
HJ, Mulsant BH, Reynolds CF 3rd, et al. Pathways linking late-life depression
to persistent cognitive impairment and dementia. Dialogues ClinNeurosci. 2008;
10(3): 345-357.
10. Monzón S, Gili M, Vives M, Serrano MJ, Bauza
N, Molina R, et al. Melancholic versus non-melancholic depression: differences
on cognitive function. A longitudinal study protocol. BMC Psychiatry. 2010; 10: 48.
11. Christensen H, Griffiths K, Mackinnon A,
Jacomb P. Quantitative review of cognitive deficits in depression and
Alzheimer-type dementia. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 1997; 3: 631-51.
12. Bassuk SS, Berkman LF, Wypij D. Depressive
symptomatology and incident cognitive decline in an elderly community sample.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1998; 55: 1073-81.
13. Airaksinen E, Larsson M, Lundberg I, Forsell
Y. Cognitive functions in depressive disorders; evidence from a
population-based study. Psychol Med. 2004; 34: 83-91.
14. Köhler S, Thomas AJ, Lloyd A, Barber R,
Almeida OP, O'Brien JT. White matter hyperintensities, cortisol levels, brain
atrophy and continuing cognitive deficits in late-life depression. Br J Psychiatry.
2010; 196: 143-9.
15. Lupien SJ, McEwen BS, Gunnar MR, Heim C.
Effects of stress throughout the lifespan on the brain, behaviour and
cognition. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2009; 10, 434-45.
16. Gorwood P, Corruble E, Falissard B, and
Goodwin GM. Toxic Effects of Depression on Brain Function: Impairment of
Delayed Recall and the Cumulative Length of Depressive Disorder in a Large
Sample of Depressed Outpatients. Am J Psychiatry 2008; 165: 731-9.
17. Austin MP, Mitchell P, Goodwin GM. Cognitive
deficits in depression; possible implication for functional neuropathology. Br
J Psychiatry. 2001; 178: 200-6.
18. Pereira
AM, Tiemensma J, Romijn JA. Neuropsychiatric Disorders in Cushing’s Syndrome.
Neuroendocrinology. 2010; 92(Suppl 1): 65-70.
19. Balbo
M, Leproult
R, Cauter
EV. Impact of Sleep and Its Disturbances on Hypothalamo-Pituitary-Adrenal Axis Activity. Int J
Endocrinol. 2010; 2010: 759234. Epub 2010 Jun 9.
20. Wolf OT, Buss C. Effect of Chronic Stress on
Cognitive Function through Life. State-of-Science Review: SR-E20. Mental
Capital and Wellbeing; 2009.
21. Jovanovic V, Pekic S, Stojanovic M, Tasic G,
Djurovic B, Soldatovic I, et al. Neuroendocrine dysfunction in patients
recovering from subarachnoid hemorrhage. Hormones. 2010; 9(3): 235-44.
22. Lucassen
PJ, Swaab
DF. Hypothalamo-Pituitary-Adrenal Axis, Stress and Depression. Encyclopedia
of Neuroscience. 2009; Part 8, 1892-1895, DOI:
10.1007/978-3-540-29678-2_2320.
23. McEwen BS. Glucocorticoids and hippocampus:
receptors in search of a function. In: Ganten D, Pfaff D, editors. Adrenal
Actions on Brain. New York:
Springer-Verlag; 1982.
24. Tiemensma J, Kokshoorn NE, Biermasz NR,
Keijser BJ, Wassenaar MJ, Middelkoop HA, et al. Subtle Cognitive Impairments in Patients with Long-Term Cure of Cushing’s Disease.
The Journal of clinical endocrinology and metabolism. 2010; PMID: 20371667.
25. Peavy GM, Salmon DP, Jacobson MW, Hervey A,
Gamst AC, Wolfson T, et al. Effects of Chronic Stress on Memory Decline in
Cognitively Normal and Mildly Impaired Older Adults. Am J Psychiatry. 2009;
166:1384-91.
26. Ottowitz W E, Deckersbach T, Cary R. Savage
C R, Lindquist M A, Dougherty DD. Neural Correlates of Strategic Processes
Underlying Episodic Memory in Women with Major Depression: A 15O-PET Study. J
Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2010; 22: 218-30.
27. Frodl T, Meisenzahl EM, Zetzsche T, Born C,
Groll C, Jäger M, et al. Hippocampal changes in patients with a first episode
of major depression. Am J Psychiatry. 2002; 159: 1112-8.
28. Dingle GA, Oei TP, Young R. Mechanisms of change in negative thinking and
urinary monoamines in depressed patients during acute treatment with group cognitive
behavior therapy and antidepressant medication. Psychiatry Res. 2010; 175
(2010): 82–8.
29. Hart J Jr, Kimbrell T, Fauver P, Cherry BJ,
Pitcock J, Booe LQ, et al. Cognitive Dysfunctions Associated With PTSD:
Evidence from World War II Prisoners of War. J Neuropsychiatr Clin Neurosci.
2008; 20:309-16.
30. Ventura J, Liberman RP, Green MF, Shaner A,
Mintz J. Training and quality assurance with the Structured Clinical Interview
for DSM-IV (SCID-I\P). Psychiatry Research. 1998; 79: 163-173.
31. Loraine JA, Bell ET. Hormone assays and their clinical
application. New York,
Churchill Livingstone, 1976.
32. Hunter WM, Corrie JET. Immunoassays for
Clinical Chemistry. Edinburgh:
Churchill Livingstone; 1978.
33. Hamilton
M. A rating scale for depression. J NeurolNeurosurg Psychiatry. 1960; 23: 56-62.
34. Wechsler D. WAIS-R Manual. New York: Psychological corp,; 1981.
35. Heaton RK. Wisconsin
Card Sorting Test Manual. Odessa,
Fla, Psychological Assessment
Resources; 1981.
36. McCall RB. Fundamental Statistics of Psychology,
3rd ed. New York:
Harcourt Brace Jovanovich Inc; 1980.
37. Frude N. A guide to SPSS/PC, Houndmills:
Macmillan Education LTD; 1987.
38. Burtis CA, Ashwood R. Tietz textbook of
clinical chemistry, 3rded. Philadelphia:
WB Saunders; 1999. p.1808.
39. Schatzberg AF, Belanoff JK, Kalehzan M, Sund
B, Fleming Ficek SK. Cortisol activity and cognitive changes in psychotic major
depression. Am J Psychiatry. 2001; 158: 1612-6.
40. O'Brien JT, Lloyd A, McKeith I, Gholkar A
and Ferrier N. A longitudinal study of hippocampal volume, cortisol levels, and
cognition in older depressed subjects. Am J Psychiatry. 2004; 161: 2081-90.
41. Nelson EB, Sax KW, Strakowski SM.
Attentional performance inpatients with psychotic and nonpsychotic major
depression and schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry. 1988; 155: 137-9.
42. Landfield PW, Eldridge JC. Evolving aspects
of the glucocorticoid hypothesis of brain aging: hormonal modulation of
neuronal calcium homeostasis. Neurobiol Aging. 1994; 15: 579-88.
43. O'Brien JT. The “glucocorticoid cascade”
hypothesis in man. Br J Psychiatry. 1997; 170:199-201.
44. Merriam EP, Thase ME, Haas GL, Keshavan MS,
Sweeney JA. Prefrontal cortical dysfunction in depression determined by
Wisconsin Card Sorting Test performance. Am J Psychiatry. 1999; 156: 780-2.
45. Bierman EJ, Comijs HC, Jonker C, Beekam ATF
Effects of anxiety versus depression on cognition in later life. Am J Geriatr
Psychiatry. 2005; 13: 686-93.